#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Parent psychological wellbeing in a single-family room versus an open bay neonatal intensive care unit


Authors: Bente Silnes Tandberg aff001;  Renée Flacking aff004;  Trond Markestad aff003;  Hege Grundt aff005;  Atle Moen aff001
Authors place of work: Department of Paediatric and Adolescent Medicine, Drammen Hospital, Vestre Viken Hospital Trust, Drammen, Norway aff001;  Lovisenberg Diaconal University College, Oslo, Norway aff002;  Department of Clinical Science, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway aff003;  School of Education, Health and Social Studies, Dalarna University, Falun, Sweden aff004;  Department of Paediatrics, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway aff005;  Department of Neonatology, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway aff006
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(11)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0224488

Summary

Background

Studies of parents’ psychological well-being in single-family rooms in neonatal intensive care units have shown conflicting results.

Aims

To compare emotional distress in the form of depression, anxiety, stress and attachment scores among parents of very preterm infants cared for in a single-family rooms unit vs an open bay unit.

Study design

Prospective survey design.

Subject

Parents (132) of 77 infants born at 28 0/7–32 0/7 weeks of gestation in the two units.

Outcome measures

Duration of parental presence was recorded. Scores for depression (The Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale), anxiety (The State–Trait–Anxiety Inventory, Short Form Y), stress (The Parent Stressor Scale: neonatal intensive care unit questionnaire and The Parenting Stress Index—short form) and attachment (Maternal Postnatal Attachment Scale) measured 14 days after delivery, at discharge, expected term date and four months post-term.

Results

Parents were present 21 hours/day in the single-family room unit vs 7 hours/day in the Open bay unit. Ninety-three percent of the fathers in the single-family rooms unit were present more than 12 hours per day during the first week. Mothers in the single-family rooms had a significantly lower depression score -1.9 (95% CI: -3.6, -0.1) points from birth to four months corrected age compared to mothers in the Open bay unit, and 14% vs 52% scored above a cut-off point considered being at high risk for depression (p<0.005). Both mothers and fathers in the single-family rooms reported significantly lower stress levels during hospitalization. There were no differences between the groups for anxiety, stress or attachment scores after discharge.

Conclusion

The lower depression scores by the mothers and lower parental stress scores during hospitalization for both parents supports that single-family rooms care contribute to parents’ psychological wellbeing.

Keywords:

Neonates – Neonatal care – Psychological stress – emotions – anxiety – Mothers – depression – Infants

Introduction

Parents of preterm infants often face immediate and prolonged separation from their babies during hospitalization. The post-partum emotional response of both the mother and the infant is rooted in instincts programmed by evolution to secure survival and safety of the mammalian off-spring, and separation may induce distress and fear in both[1, 2]. Compared to other mammalian species, the brain of the human newborn is larger and more adaptable, but also particularly immature and dependent on caregiving behaviours and a nurturing environment [3]. There is increasing evidence that early experience and stimulation may influence long-term outcomes and the mechanism may at least partly be related to the rapid development of the brain during infancy and most pronounced in infants born preterm [4]. Stressors during the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) hospitalisation may affect regulation of the hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis, which is our central stress response regulating system, as well as general brain development [5, 6]. The bonding and interaction between infants and their mothers are also important for healthy developmental trajectories [3]. Over the last two decades the principles of family-centred care have gradually been implemented in the care of premature and sick newborn infants [7, 8] and in 2018, the European Foundation for the Care of Newborn Infants (EFCNI) launched the European standards of care for newborn health, defining family-centred care and a physical environment that allows extensive parental presence and participation as the European standard of care for hospitalized newborn infants [9, 10]. Parental presence also brings care in accordance with the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child, Article 7, acknowledging the infant’s right to be cared for by his or her parents [11]. However, there are large variation between units in parental presence and involvement, also in units claiming to work in accordance with family-centred principles [12, 13].

The number of NICU’s with a single patient or single-family room design (SFR) is growing. The medical and psychological benefits of including parents in care have been well documented [14], and one study in particular has provided evidence for both short- and long-term medical benefits of SFR care [1517]. Parents’ participation in care may also be beneficial for parents’ own mental health [14, 16]. However, Pineda et al. [12] and Domanico et al. (13) showed an increase in parental stress and isolation when infants were treated in single-patient rooms. Even though parental presence increased with a SFR design, the time of parental presence in these studies was low, in particular the time providing active care, holding and skin-to-skin contact (SSC) [18, 19]. Pineda et al. have even indicated adverse findings on MRI and neurodevelopmental outcome at two years after care in single-patient rooms [20]. However, in this unit single room care was carried out with very limited parental presence and family participation compared to what is commonly seen in a Scandinavian NICUs [21]. Although most NICU professionals may acknowledge that parent participation is warranted, there is no consensus on how much presence and active participation in care parents can and wish to provide. It is well documented that parents of preterm infants may experience mixed emotions, causing symptoms of stress, anxiety and/or depression [22, 23]. In this study, we have used parents’ self-reports of depression and anxiety, stress and negative influence on attachment as indicators of emotional distress.

Differences in parental outcomes may be influenced by external policy factors such as rights to parental leave and access to health insurance, and by socio-economic differences that are not directly observable by parents or the NICU staff. In addition, differences in infant morbidity between studies may contribute. No studies have explored the effects on emotional distress when both parents live with their infant all or most of the day from birth to discharge, and we therefore designed a controlled study of parents’ emotional distress in two different units providing care in accordance with the principles of family-centred care. One unit had a SFR design; the other was an old unit with an open bay (OB) design.

We hypothesized that parents participating actively in care through continuous presence in a SFR unit did not experience more emotional distress than parents in an OB unit who spent less time with their infant.

Materials and methods

We have previously reported effects of SFR design on parental presence, infant growth trajectories, morbidity, medical procedures and nutrition [24]. In the present study, we report parents’ emotional reactions to continuous presence, using questionnaires to screen for the risk of depression, anxiety, stress and attachment, and we provide a more in-depth description of parental presence. Both participating units were located in maternity hospitals and provided care until discharge.

The units

In 2012, the NICU at Vestre Viken Hospital Trust, Drammen, Norway, was established as a SFR, allowing parents to stay with their infant day and night from birth to discharge and to participate as primary caregivers. The unit provided care from birth for infants with gestational age (GA) ≥ 28.0 weeks and admits approximately 450 infants in 17 beds annually. Each room has two different areas; one infant-area with a place for the incubator or cot, sink, nursing table, and equipment (CPAP, pumps, ventilators), in addition to a parent-area with two high-quality hospital beds (105 cm wide electrically adjustable). Separate bathrooms are included in all SFRs. At day time, there is no physical separation between the parent and infant area and equipment are mounted on flexible arms allowing easy and secure transfer of the infant from the incubator to the parents’ bed without disconnecting medical equipment. During night time, parents can close flexible folding doors to the sleeping area, while nurses still have direct access to the infant without interrupting parents (pictures of the SFR unit are presented as supplement, S1a–S1c Picture). All meals were provided without cost to both parents. At the time of the study, parents had access to a psychologist working part time at the unit and to weekly parent meetings with other parents. The unit was staffed with five consultants having 50% of their clinical service in the unit and 62 registered nurses of whom 24% were specialists in intensive care, paediatrics, or neonatal nursing. Parents were present and participated actively during daily rounds.

The OB unit was located at Haukeland University Hospital in Bergen, Norway, and provided care from birth for infants with GA ≥ 23.0 weeks. The OB unit was built in 1979 and underwent no subsequent major changes. It had 21 beds and admitted approximately 500 infants per year. Except for one single-bed room used for high-intensive or end-of-life care, the unit had two rooms; one for intensive- and intermediate care infants and one for care in cots before discharge to home. The rooms were crowded, but one reclining armchair could be placed between incubators or cots, and screens could be placed around the family to provide some privacy. The parents had unlimited access at all hours, but they could not stay overnight in the unit. Mothers were offered accommodation in another building at the hospital area after discharge from the maternity ward. Free meals were provided only for mothers. A psychologist was available upon special request. The number of neonatologists in full time position was 3.5 and 64% of nurses were specialist nurses. Parents were not routinely involved in medical rounds.

Although the facilities available for parents to room in were different, both units had an explicit policy of allowing parents unlimited access and to stay with their infant for as long as they wanted. SSC was strongly encouraged in both units. Both units encouraged and guided mothers to provide breastfeeding from day one.

Norway has extensive social benefits related to pregnancy and birth. Health care insurance is publicly funded, hospital care is free of costs and both parents are allowed full job-leave with compensation for salary-loss during hospitalization with their infant. Parents also have 48 weeks of fully paid parental leave shared between them after discharge from the NICU.

Participants

Parents of infants born at 28 0/7–32 0/7 weeks of gestation with the mothers’ address in the hospitals’ respective catchment areas were eligible for inclusion. Infants with congenital malformations or major complications (intraventricular haemorrhage grade III/ IV or surgically treated necrotizing enterocolitis) and infants with birth weight less than 800 grams were excluded. We also excluded infants if one or both parents suffered from a major mental illness or did not understand Norwegian language, infants of mothers who had taken illicit drugs or were on methadone during pregnancy and infants in the custody of the Child Protection Services from birth. Both parents received oral and written information about the study, and they were included if both gave written consent by the end of the second day post-partum. In the SFR unit, 60 parents of 35 neonates were included and in the OB unit 72 parents of 42 neonates were included consecutively. Inclusion to the study started on May 1, 2014 and ended on July 31, 2016 as the OB unit was moved to another building with better facilities (Fig 1).

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of participant recruitment.
Flow diagram of participant recruitment.

The study was approved by the Norwegian Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics and registered at ClinicalTrials.Gov (NCT 02452580).

Data collection

For each infant, both parents prospectively recorded time present in the unit with their infant and the duration of SSC on the mother’s or father’s bare chest. From birth to postmenstrual age of 34 weeks, both periods were registered each day in a closeness diary lying next to the infant. Twins had separate diaries. Continuous presence was defined as presence for more than 12 hours a day for each parent.

Parents were asked to complete a set of questionnaires at 14 days post-partum, at discharge, at term date and at four months after term date. If one parent did not participate in a follow-up consultation after discharge, the questionnaires were brought home with the participating parent along with a stamped envelope and returned by post to the project manager. For twins parents answered one set of questionnaire.

1) The Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) [25] aims to identify depressive symptoms in pregnant women or women who have recently given birth. The EPDS is validated for use in a Norwegian population [26, 27]. The range of score is 0–30 and the score increases with a increasing symptoms. We applied a cut off score ≥ 13 giving a sensitivity of 77% and specificity of 94% in detecting symptoms of depression [28].

2) The State–Trait–Anxiety Inventory, Short Form Y (STAI SF) measures symptoms of anxiety in adults [29]. The short version contains six statements, three items with anxiety present and three with anxiety absent, which the respondents rate on a scale from 1 to 4 [30]. The range of the total STAI score is 20–80 and it increases with increasing symptoms. Scores below 36 is considered normal [31]. STAI SF has demonstrated reliability and validity in study samples of parents with sick infants [32].

3) The Parental Stressor Scale: NICU (PSS: NICU) [33, 34] measures stress experienced by parents during hospitalization related to alterations in their parental role, the appearance and behaviour of their child, and sights and sounds of the unit. Parents are asked to rate items on a five-point scale ranging from "not at all stressful" to "extremely stressful". “Sights and sounds of the environment” and “Infant’s appearance” are scored as one sub-scale, with scores ranging from 20 to 100. “Parental role alteration” has a range of scores from 7 to 35. The tool has been shown to predict depressive symptoms [34] and a moderate correlation with state anxiety [35] and has also been validated for a European population [36].

4) Parenting Stress Index (PSI- SF). The short form (36 questions) of PSI is a widely used clinical and research self-report questionnaire to identify stress due to parental factors or deviant development of the child [37, 38]. The questionnaire includes a parent domain (i.e. social isolation, attachment to the child, health, role restriction, depression and partner) and a child domain (i.e. distractibility/hyperactivity, adaptability, how demanding the child is perceived to be, mood and acceptance). The total score ranges from 18 to 90 and higher scores indicate higher levels of parent-related stress. A total score between 52 and 90 is considered to represent a high-risk level, whereas scores from 18 to 44 are considered low-risk/normal [39].

5) The Maternal Postnatal Attachment Scale (MPAS) evaluates the mother’s subjective feeling of attachment (“the emotional tie”) to the infant (40). In this study, fathers also were asked to complete the MPAS. The instrument consists of 19 statements referring to three different factors: patience and tolerance, pleasure in interaction and affection and pride. The respondents indicate to what extent (always, very often, often, sometimes) the statements match their perception. The possible range of scores is 19 to 95, higher scores indicating more attachment. At term date, the mean normal score was 83 (range 56–95) and at four months post-term it was 85 (range 59–95) [40].

The STAI and MPAS tools were translated into Norwegian with forward and backward translation. For PSS: NICU, a former Norwegian translation was used.

Questionnaires were not returned by: 2% and 13% of the mothers and fathers, respectively, at day 14; by 3% and 16% at discharge; by 18% and 27% at term; and by 20% and 17%, respectively, at four-month corrected age. There were no significant differences between the two groups in the number of unreturned questionnaires.

Statistics

The SFR and OB units were compared by independent sample t-tests, Mann-Whitney tests or Pearson’s chi-square tests, according to distribution of the data. Two baseline characteristics were unequally distributed in the groups (mode of delivery and education). Therefore, in addition to the main explanatory variable (the SFR or the OB unit), mean differences in outcome measures (parents’ answers to the questionnaire) were analysed with a linear mixed model. This model included repeated measurements and thereby the effects of time, and took into account the correlation structure and dependency between the repeated measurements. The model treated each of the measurements (scores from the different questionnaires from birth to four months after expected term date) as level one and the individual parent as level two. This is a two-level model with fixed effects for unit, time, mode of delivery (vaginal or caesarean section) and parental education (elementary, high school and college/university). In the mixed model, we used the autoregressive covariance structure (AR1) because the correlations between adjacent time points were higher than the correlations between measurements at time points further apart. Model assumptions (collinearity, residuals and outliers) were thoroughly checked. Results were given as an estimate of the mean difference between the OB and SFR units, adjusted for confounders with corresponding 95% confidence intervals. For the questionnaires, EPDS, STAI and MPAS missing values of the inventory were replaced by the mean value from remaining items when no more than one was missing from the sub scale.

One item on each of the two different sub-scales in the PSS: NICU (“Sights and sounds of the environment and Infant appearance”; and “Parental role alteration”) were systematically missing because of a technical failure when distributing this questionnaire to parents in both units. The two items (“My baby’s unusual or abnormal breathing patterns” and “Not being able to hold my baby when I want”) were replaced by the remaining items on each of the sub-scales, after agreement with the author, Dr M. Miles (e-mail correspondence dated 05.09.2018). For the PSI, which contains several domains, answers were replaced with the mean value from the other score within each domain if no more than two item answers were missing from the parent domain and no more than one item from the child domain. The statistical significance was set at a p-value of <0.05.

Mean differences between the SFR and OB units in duration of parental presence and SSC until postmenstrual age of 34 weeks were determined in linear regression analyses. The main exposure was the unit (SFR or OB), and the outcomes were adjusted for postmenstrual age at birth, mode of delivery (vaginal or caesarean section) and parents’ education (elementary/high school or college/university). Analyses of parental presence were performed separately for mothers and fathers, with an additional analysis of the cumulative parental presence and SSC for each infant. All analyses were done in SPSS Statistics version 25 (IBM, Inc., Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

The SFR parents had a lower level of education, a higher proportion of the infants were delivered by caesarean section and their mean GA was slightly higher (Table 1).

Tab. 1. Characteristics of the families and infants cared for in the single-family room (SFR) and open-bay (OB) units presented as means (SDs) or number (%) within each unit.
Characteristics of the families and infants cared for in the single-family room (SFR) and open-bay (OB) units presented as means (SDs) or number (%) within each unit.

During the first week, both parents in the SFR unit were present for a mean of 20 hours per day, while the parents in the OB unit were present for a mean of four hours (Fig 2).

Fig. 2. Parental presence first week.
Parental presence first week.
Mean hours of daily presence during first week of hospitalisation by mothers and fathers in the SFR unit and OB unit.

Eighty-seven percent of the mothers and 93% of the fathers in the SFR unit were continuously present (>12 hours per day), compared to none in the OB unit (Table 1). From birth until postmenstrual age of 34 weeks, parents in SFR maintained an average continued presence of 21 hours for mothers and 16 hours for fathers, compared, respectively, with seven hours and five hours in the OB unit. The respective mean daily hours of providing SSC from the second week were six and four hours (p<0.001) (Table 1).

The EPDS scores were lower for mothers in the SFR than in the OB unit from birth to four months corrected age, and the estimated difference for the period was -1.9 (95% CI: -3.6, -0.1) (Table 2). Table showing all covariates examined in the linear mixed model of repeated measurements are provided as supplement, S1 Table.

Tab. 2. Repeated measurements of depression, anxiety, stress and attachment by parents, presented as adjusted mean difference examined in linear mixed model.
Repeated measurements of depression, anxiety, stress and attachment by parents, presented as adjusted mean difference examined in linear mixed model.

The difference between units was most pronounced during hospitalization, when 14% scored at a level indicating symptoms of depression in the SFR unit, as opposed to 52% in the OB unit (p<0.005) (Table 3).

Tab. 3. Mothers’ and fathers’ scores of depression, anxiety, stress and attachment, presented as means (SDs), median [Q1, Q3] or number (%) within each unit.
Mothers’ and fathers’ scores of depression, anxiety, stress and attachment, presented as means (SDs), median [Q1, Q3] or number (%) within each unit.

During hospitalization, the SFR parents scored 8 points lower on the STAI-SF questionnaire (Table 3). Parents’ scores decreased to levels considered normal in both units at discharge and in the mixed model there was no significant difference between the units (Table 2).

Both SFR parents scored lower on stress related to “Sights and sounds of the environment” and “Infant’s appearance”, a mean difference of -5.0 (95% CI: -9.4, -0.6) by mothers and -5.3 (95% CI: -9.5, -1.1) by fathers. Also in regard to stress related to “Parental role alteration”, the SFR parents scored lower, with a mean difference of -5.2 (95% CI: -8.7, -1.6) by mothers and -7.2 (95% CI: -10.3, -4.2) reported by fathers.

From term date, there were no differences in PSI-SF scores between the groups in any of the sub-scales (Table 2). Parents in both units reported average scores just above the high-risk level (score of 52–90) and total stress scores remained in the lower part of the range defined as high-risk level (Table 3).

There were no significant differences between the units on the MPAS sum scores (Table 2). Mothers and fathers in both units scored high on parental attachment (Table 3).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first controlled study of emotional distress in a setting with documented continuous parental presence during their infant’s medical care. Emotional distress did not increase, and the risk of depression and stress were actually decreased among parents in the SFR unit compared to the OB unit. Of particular interest is the extensive presence by fathers in the SFR unit from the day of birth. Such extensive active participation throughout the infants’ stay by fathers has not previously been reported. Studies of fathers’ role and involvement in care and their contribution to the social–emotional development of preterm infants has just started to emerge [41, 42]; however, knowledge of how fathers increased involvement contributes to and affects the family is still limited [43].

Emotional distress was measured with five different questionnaires covering different aspects of emotional reactions and disturbance in attachment. We consider the consistency in the finding of no increased emotional distress in the SFR compared to the OB unit across the panel of questionnaires to strengthen our conclusion. Also, the uniform selection of patients, the similar right to health care services and parental leave, the clearly defined differences in design and the large differences in time parents were present in the two units strengthens the validity of the conclusion. The units were located more than 400 km apart, and they did not cooperate beyond this specific project. Therefore, a spill-over effect or negative expectations among participants, which may be a major challenge in true randomized controlled trials, seems rather unlikely [44].

We have previously shown that the study populations did not differ significantly in terms of morbidity and practices related to treatment and nutrition [24]. Although both units were the only units providing care for the eligible infants in their respective geographical areas and provided the same medical and nursing care, we cannot exclude unrecognized confounders related to care culture and practices. The OB unit also provides care for smaller and sicker infants, which could increase the general level of a stress in the unit. Studies using a quasi-randomization [20] or a before-and-after design with asynchrony in time between the study groups [15, 16] are also prone to the same and other confounders.

Lester et al. found that the effects of SFR were largely mediated through increased maternal involvement, breastfeeding and developmental care in the SFR unit. Optimizing facilities for parents of preterm infants in the NICU and thereby increasing parental presence and involvement may contribute to improved long-term outcomes [45]. Parents may provide unique sensory stimulation to their infants through SSC [46], talk and singing [47, 48]. The possibilities of such positive stimulation are better when parents are present around the clock compared to a few hours of visiting each day.

In our study, gestational age was higher and morbidity lower than in other studies reporting effects of SFR design [16, 49]. This may influence both the levels of distress and the extent of parental presence. In our experience, parents do not disappear or back out when the infant’s condition is deteriorating. Unfortunately we do not have data on maternal health (e.g., pre-eclampsia) before preterm delivery or about the parents’ previous mental health status. Both factors could potentially have some impact on the outcome measures, but there is no obvious reason why this should differ between the two study groups.

Regarding the difference in depression scores in mothers it is difficult to state a clinically relevant effect size precisely, but it has been proposed to be around four points [50]. Our results showed a difference of six points at day 14. It is relevant to speculate about an association between time spent per day by mothers with their infant and the risk of developing depressive symptoms. In the SFR unit, mothers were present daily three times longer than in the OB unit (21 vs. 7 hours). From a biological and evolutionary perspective, not being allowed or able to protect and take maternal responsibility for the infant would be expected to cause emotional distress and may explain the report of more depressive symptoms (52%) by mothers in the OB unit. However, only 6% of the fathers in the OB unit scored above the cut-off of >13 points at day 14, indicating a difference in vulnerability between mothers and fathers immediately after preterm birth.

Others have documented an increased burden of emotional distress on fathers after preterm birth [51, 52]. The extensive presence of fathers in SFR’s throughout the stay, with an average of 20 hours daily for the first three days, may provide additional emotional support for mothers who have been initially incapacitated and recovering from complications of pregnancy. How fathers’ biological emotional responses are programmed and developed towards their preterm infant has not yet been sufficiently explored [53]. We found that fathers did not report depressive symptoms, and this finding was similar in both units.

Both groups scored in the lower range for anxiety, indicating that this was not a predominant symptom among parents in any of these units. Infants included in the study carried a low risk for both short- and long term severe adverse outcomes, and this may have contributed to the low scores on anxiety.

Although stress among NICU parents is well documented by others [23, 54, 55], we found parents’ average stress scores to be in the lower range. Parents of preterm infants are undoubtedly prone to stress, but the effect sizes are small in populations with low morbidity and higher gestational ages [56]. Nevertheless, the differences in stress scores between the units were significant during hospitalization. The mean stress scores were more than five points higher for the mothers, and seven points for fathers in OB unit compared to the SFR unit, which could be considered clinically relevant. A previous study of the two units found that parents in the SFR unit gave higher scores on emotional support and participation [57]. Increased satisfaction with care may not necessarily decrease emotional distress, but a possible causality between the two deserves further research.

Pineda et al found slightly increased stress in mothers of infants hospitalized in single rooms and argued that stress was related to isolation, lack of support from other mothers, in addition to an increased feeling of obligation and responsibility of the infant. However the authors also hypothesised that the large variation in visitation could be associated with other factors like socioeconomic status and maternal health, and that they may have a larger impact on maternal stress than time present in a single-room [18].

In the SFR unit parents were included in daily rounds and may therefore represent the best continuity in the care of their infant [57]. When parents are involved and allowed unrestricted access, they participate actively in shared decision making at an informed and competent level, based on their knowledge of the infant. Most parents in the SFR unit are present also during night-time. They rarely leave the infant to the staff, and their continuous presence allows them to provide closeness and care immediately at the cues of the infant. This may reinforce parents’ feeling of being in control and provide stress relief. Aagaard et al. found mothering of a preterm infant to be a developmental process nurtured by close relationship with the infant [54]. The ability to be close to the infant is indeed enhanced in the SFR unit, and this may trigger positive emotions [58]. The questionnaire, PSS: NICU, may also predict depressive symptoms, and as such confirms the differences between the units from the EPDS scores. Still, at term date and at four months post-term, parents in both units scored just above the lower limit for high risk on the PSI questionnaire, without any difference between the units. This could question the validity of the results of the PSS: NICU, but it may just as well reflect stress experienced during the transition from hospital to home. Using a modified version of the PSI questionnaire, Flacking et al. found, in accordance with our findings, no overall effect of co-care vs. no co-care on stress, but reported more stress on a sub-scale related to feelings of incompetence among the mothers as a result of being unable to provide co-care [59].

Preterm infants can, for obvious reasons, only express their distress indirectly, through behavioural signals and physiological instability [60]. The long-term negative effects of infant stress during NICU care are also starting to emerge through follow-up studies with impaired neurodevelopment and psychological outcomes [55, 61]. The majority of effective non-pharmacological interventions to reduce infant pain and distress require active parental participation [16, 6265]. Provided with facilities supporting presence in the SFR unit, parents chose to be present for most of the day and night. We therefore document that extensive parental participation is possible without increasing parents’ emotional distress; indeed, it seems to be reduced by continuous presence. Most research on the effect of positive stimulation and parental presence in the NICU has been conducted in preterm infants, but the same basic psychological needs mediated through parental closeness are present also for severely ill infants born at term. Parental presence and their vulnerability to psychological distress may be influenced by external factors such as differences in health care financing, a social welfare system compensating presence economically as recognized also by others [18]. To rebuild a NICU into SFR facilities are costly, potentially there could be cheaper interventions (e.g., frequent psychological support) to increase parents’ psychological wellness. However, a large and increasing evidence based knowledge of the medical and psychological benefits of parent–infant closeness in the NICU may point at near-continuous parental presence as one of the most underestimated interventions available in NICU care. A society and health care system adopting a policy allowing continuous parental presence takes a major step towards the goal of providing care at the premises of the patient and in accordance with the highest medical, legal and ethical standards.

Conclusion

This study shows that continuous presence of both parents of infants hospitalised in a SFR NICU can be achieved without increasing parental distress. In addition, the risk of depression and stress decreased during hospitalization with potential long-term positive effects on parental well-being. Providing a NICU design that enables parents to stay continuously may also be beneficial for long-term outcomes of the infants. A physical design of the NICU facilitating the implementation of evidence-based practice of parental presence and participation should therefore be considered superior to a design limiting these possibilities.

Supporting information

S1 Picture [jpg]
Patient and parents area in the SFR unit.

S2 Picture [jpg]
Patient area in the SFR unit.

S3 Picture [jpg]
Illustration photo of patient and parent in the SFR unit.

S1 Table [docx]
Table showing all covariates examined in the linear mixed model.

S1 File [docx]
Protocol.

S2 File [pdf]
TREND Checklist.


Zdroje

1. Bergman NJ, Ludwig R, Westrup B, Welch M. Nurturescience versus neuroscience: A case for rethinking perinatal mother–infant behaviors and relationship. Birth defects research. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdr2.1529

2. Bergman NJ. Birth practices: Maternal‐neonate separation as a source of toxic stress. Birth defects research. 2019. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1530

3. Provenzi L, Scotto di Minico G, Giusti L, Guida E, Müller M. Disentangling the dyadic dance: theoretical, methodological and outcomes systematic review of mother-infant dyadic processes. Frontiers in psychology. 2018;9:348. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2961594729615947

4. Volpe JJ. Dysmaturation of Premature Brain: Importance, Cellular Mechanisms, and Potential Interventions. Pediatr Neurol. 2019;95:42–66. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2019.02.016 30975474

5. Montirosso R, Provenzi L. Implications of epigenetics and stress regulation on research and developmental care of preterm infants. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2015;44(2):174–82. doi: 10.1111/1552-6909.12559 25712710

6. Mooney-Leber SM, Brummelte S. Neonatal pain and reduced maternal care: Early-life stressors interacting to impact brain and behavioral development. Neuroscience. 2017;342:21–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.05.001 27167085

7. Westrup B. Family-centered developmentally supportive care. NeoReviews. 2014;15(8):e325–e35.

8. Roue JM, Kuhn P, Lopez Maestro M, Maastrup RA, Mitanchez D, Westrup B, et al. Eight principles for patient-centred and family-centred care for newborns in the neonatal intensive care unit. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102(4):F364–F8. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2016-312180 28420745

9. Moen A, Hallberg B. EFCNI, European Standards of Care for Newborn Health: Core principles of NICU design to promote family-centred care 2018 https://newborn-health-standards.org/nicu-design-family-centred-care/.

10. Westrup B KPea. EFCNI, European Standards of Care for Newborn Health: Family access. 2018 https://newborn-health-standards.org/family-access/

11. UN. Convention on the Rights of the Child: United Nations Human Rights Office of the high Commissioner; 1989 https://www.ohchr.org/en/professionalinterest/pages/crc.aspx.

12. Raiskila S, Axelin A, Toome L, Caballero S, Tandberg BS, Montirosso R, et al. Parents’ presence and parent–infant closeness in 11 neonatal intensive care units in six European countries vary between and within the countries. Acta Paediatrica. 2017:n/a-n/a. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apa.13798

13. Pallas-Alonso CR, Losacco V, Maraschini A, Greisen G, Pierrat V, Warren I, et al. Parental involvement and kangaroo care in European neonatal intensive care units: a policy survey in eight countries. Pediatr Crit Care Med. 2012;13(5):568–77. doi: 10.1097/PCC.0b013e3182417959 22760425

14. O’Brien K, Robson K, Bracht M, Cruz M, Lui K, Alvaro R, et al. Effectiveness of Family Integrated Care in neonatal intensive care units on infant and parent outcomes: a multicentre, multinational, cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2018;2(4):245–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30039-30169298

15. Lester BM, Hawes K, Abar B, Sullivan M, Miller R, Bigsby R, et al. Single-family room care and neurobehavioral and medical outcomes in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):754–60. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-4252 25246623

16. Lester BM, Salisbury AL, Hawes K, Dansereau LM, Bigsby R, Laptook A, et al. 18-Month Follow-Up of Infants Cared for in a Single-Family Room Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. J Pediatr. 2016;177:84–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.06.069 27470693

17. Vohr B, McGowan E, McKinley L, Tucker R, Keszler L, Alksninis B. Differential Effects of the Single-Family Room Neonatal Intensive Care Unit on 18- to 24-Month Bayley Scores of Preterm Infants. J Pediatr. 2017;185:42–8 e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.01.05628238479

18. Pineda RG, Stransky KE, Rogers C, Duncan MH, Smith GC, Neil J, et al. The single-patient room in the NICU: maternal and family effects. J Perinatol. 2012;32(7):545–51. doi: 10.1038/jp.2011.144 22031044

19. Domanico R, Davis DK, Coleman F, Davis BO Jr. Documenting the NICU design dilemma: parent and staff perceptions of open ward versus single family room units. J Perinatol. 2010;30(5):343–51. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/jp.2009.19520072132

20. Pineda RG, Neil J, Dierker D, Smyser CD, Wallendorf M, Kidokoro H, et al. Alterations in brain structure and neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants hospitalized in different neonatal intensive care unit environments. J Pediatr. 2014;164(1):52–60 e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.08.04724139564

21. Raiskila S, Axelin A, Toome L, Caballero S, Tandberg BS, Montirosso R, et al. Parents’ presence and parent-infant closeness in 11 neonatal intensive care units in six European countries vary between and within the countries. Acta Paediatr. 2017;106(6):878–88. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2823515228235152

22. Misund AR, Nerdrum P, Diseth TH. Mental health in women experiencing preterm birth. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2014;14(1):263. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-14-263 25107462

23. Prouhet PM, Gregory MR, Russell CL, Yaeger LH. Fathers’ Stress in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit: A Systematic Review. Adv Neonatal Care. 2018;18(2):105–20. doi: 10.1097/ANC.0000000000000472 29595549

24. Tandberg BS, Froslie KF, Markestad T, Flacking R, Grundt H, Moen A. Single-family room design in the neonatal intensive care unit did not improve growth. Acta Paediatr. 2019;108(6):1028–35. doi: 10.1111/apa.14746 30729563

25. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150(6):782–6. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782

26. Eberhard-Gran M, Eskild A, Tambs K, Schei B, Opjordsmoen S. The Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale: validation in a Norwegian community sample. Nord J Psychiatry. 2001;55(2):113–7. https://doi.org/10.1080/0803948011768411802908

27. Eberhard‐Gran M, Eskild A, Tambs K, Opjordsmoen S, Ove Samuelsen S. Review of validation studies of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2001;104(4):243–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2001.00187.x11722298

28. Rubertsson C, Börjesson K, Berglund A, Josefsson A, Sydsjö G. The Swedish validation of Edinburgh postnatal depression scale (EPDS) during pregnancy. Nordic journal of psychiatry. 2011;65(6):414–8. https://doi.org/10.3109/08039488.2011.59060621728782

29. Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (form Y)(" self-evaluation questionnaire"). 1983. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0943

30. Marteau TM, Bekker HL. The development of a six‐item short‐form of the state scale of the Spielberger State—Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1992;31(3):301–6.

31. Bekker HL, Legare F, Stacey D, O’Connor A, Lemyre L. Is anxiety a suitable measure of decision aid effectiveness: a systematic review? Patient Education and Counseling. 2003;50(3):255–62. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(03)00045-4 12900095

32. Tluczek A, Henriques JB, Brown RL. Support for the reliability and validity of a six-item state anxiety scale derived from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Journal of nursing measurement. 2009;17(1):19. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2776769/ 19902657

33. Miles MS, Funk SG, Carlson J. Parental Stressor Scale: neonatal intensive care unit. Nurs Res. 1993;42(3):148–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006199-199305000-00005 8506163

34. Miles MS, Holditch-Davis D, Schwartz TA, Scher M. Depressive symptoms in mothers of prematurely born infants. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2007;28(1):36–44. doi: 10.1097/01.DBP.0000257517.52459.7a 17353730

35. Franck LS, Cox S, Allen A, Winter I. Measuring neonatal intensive care unit-related parental stress. J Adv Nurs. 2005;49(6):608–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.03336.x 15737221

36. Månsson C, Jakobsson U, Lundqvist P. Translation and psychometric evaluation of a Swedish version of the parental stressor scale PSS: NICU. Scandinavian journal of caring sciences. 2016;30(1):193–201. https://doi.org/10.1111/scs.1221725918977

37. Abidin RR. Parenting Stress Index: Manual, Administration Booklet,[and] Research Update. 1983.

38. Abidin RR, Flens JR, Austin WG. Parenting Stress Index. Forensic uses of clinical assessment instruments. 1995:297–328.

39. Kornør H, & Martinussen, M. PSI: Parenting Stress Index, 3. versjon. I H. Kornør (red.), PsykTestBARN. Hentet fra http://www.psyktest.no/PSI.63155.cms. 2011.

40. Condon JT, Corkindale CJ. The assessment of parent-to-infant attachment: Development of a self-report questionnaire instrument. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 1998;16(1):57–76. doi: 10.1080/02646839808404558

41. Feeley N, Waitzer E, Sherrard K, Boisvert L, Zelkowitz P. Fathers’ perceptions of the barriers and facilitators to their involvement with their newborn hospitalised in the neonatal intensive care unit. J Clin Nurs. 2013;22(3–4):521–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2012.04231.x 23186388

42. McMahon GE, Spencer-Smith MM, Pace CC, Spittle AJ, Stedall P, Richardson K, et al. Influence of Fathers’ Early Parenting on the Development of Children Born Very Preterm and Full Term. The Journal of pediatrics. 2019;205:195–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.09.073 30448013

43. Sabnis A, Fojo S, Nayak SS, Lopez E, Tarn DM, Zeltzer L. Reducing parental trauma and stress in neonatal intensive care: systematic review and meta-analysis of hospital interventions. J Perinatol. 2019;39(3):375–86. doi: 10.1038/s41372-018-0310-9 30659239

44. Shields L, Zhou H, Pratt J, Taylor M, Hunter J, Pascoe E. Family-centred care for hospitalised children aged 0–12 years. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012;10:CD004811. https://www.cochranelibrary.com/cdsr/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD004811.pub3

45. Belsky J, de Haan M. Annual Research Review: Parenting and children’s brain development: the end of the beginning. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52(4):409–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02281.x

46. Ludington-Hoe SM. Skin-to-Skin Contact: A Comforting Place With Comfort Food. MCN Am J Matern Child Nurs. 2015;40(6):359–66; quiz E23–4. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2628094726280947

47. Provenzi L, Broso S, Montirosso R. Do mothers sound good? A systematic review of the effects of maternal voice exposure on preterm infants’ development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018;88:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.009

48. Filippa M, Lordier L, De Almeida JS, Monaci MG, Darque A, Grandjean D, et al. Early vocal contact and music in the NICU: new insights into preventive interventions. Pediatric research. 2019:1. doi: 10.1038/s41390-019-0521-6

49. Pineda RG, Neil J, Dierker D, Smyser CD, Wallendorf M, Kidokoro H, et al. Alterations in brain structure and neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants hospitalized in different neonatal intensive care unit environments. The Journal of pediatrics. 2014;164(1):52–60. e2. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2413956424139564

50. Matthey S. Calculating clinically significant change in postnatal depression studies using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Journal of affective disorders. 2004;78(3):269–72. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00313-0 15013253

51. Prouhet PM, Gregory MR, Russell CL, Yaeger LH. Fathers’ stress in the neonatal intensive care unit: A systematic review. Advances in Neonatal Care. 2018;18(2):105–20. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2959554929595549

52. Provenzi L, Santoro E. The lived experience of fathers of preterm infants in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit: a systematic review of qualitative studies. J Clin Nurs. 2015;24(13–14):1784–94. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2585051825850518

53. Fisher D, Khashu M, Adama EA, Feeley N, Garfield CF, Ireland J, et al. Fathers in neonatal units: Improving infant health by supporting the baby-father bond and mother-father coparenting. Journal of Neonatal Nursing. 2018;24(6):306–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jnn.2018.08.007

54. Aagaard H, Hall EO. Mothers’ experiences of having a preterm infant in the neonatal care unit: a meta-synthesis. Journal of pediatric nursing. 2008;23(3):e26–e36. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2007.02.003 18492543

55. Lean RE, Rogers CE, Paul RA, Gerstein ED. NICU Hospitalization: Long-Term Implications on Parenting and Child Behaviors. Curr Treat Options Pediatr. 2018;4(1):49–69. doi: 10.1007/s40746-018-0112-5 29881666

56. Schappin R, Wijnroks L, Uniken Venema MM, Jongmans MJ. Rethinking stress in parents of preterm infants: a meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2013;8(2):e54992. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.005499223405105

57. Tandberg BS, Froslie KF, Flacking R, Grundt H, Lehtonen L, Moen A. Parent-Infant Closeness, Parents’ Participation, and Nursing Support in Single-Family Room and Open Bay NICUs. J Perinat Neonatal Nurs. 2018;32(4):E22–E32. doi: 10.1097/JPN.0000000000000359 30358674

58. Flacking R, Thomson G, Axelin A. Pathways to emotional closeness in neonatal units—a cross-national qualitative study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2016;16(1):170. doi: 10.1186/s12884-016-0955-3

59. Flacking R, Thomson G, Ekenberg L, Lowegren L, Wallin L. Influence of NICU co-care facilities and skin-to-skin contact on maternal stress in mothers of preterm infants. Sex Reprod Healthc. 2013;4(3):107–12. doi: 10.1016/j.srhc.2013.06.002 24041731

60. Als H. A synactive model of neonatal behavioral organization. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics. 1986;6(3–4):3–53.

61. Pineda R, Bender J, Hall B, Shabosky L, Annecca A, Smith J. Parent participation in the neonatal intensive care unit: Predictors and relationships to neurobehavior and developmental outcomes. Early Hum Dev. 2018;117:32–8. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.12.008 29275070

62. Lyngstad LT, Tandberg BS, Storm H, Ekeberg BL, Moen A. Does skin-to-skin contact reduce stress during diaper change in preterm infants? Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(4):169–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.01.01124548816

63. Philpott-Robinson K, Lane SJ, Korostenski L, Lane AE. The impact of the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit on sensory and developmental outcomes in infants born preterm: A scoping review. British journal of occupational therapy. 2017;80(8):459–69. doi: 10.1177/0308022617709761

64. Puthussery S, Chutiyami M, Tseng PC, Kilby L, Kapadia J. Effectiveness of early intervention programs for parents of preterm infants: a meta-review of systematic reviews. BMC Pediatr. 2018;18(1):223. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-018-1205-929986683

65. Pineda R, Guth R, Herring A, Reynolds L, Oberle S, Smith J. Enhancing sensory experiences for very preterm infants in the NICU: an integrative review. Journal of Perinatology. 2016;37(4):323. doi: 10.1038/jp.2016.179 27763631


Článok vyšiel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 11
Najčítanejšie tento týždeň
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvýšte si kvalifikáciu online z pohodlia domova

Aktuální možnosti diagnostiky a léčby litiáz
nový kurz
Autori: MUDr. Tomáš Ürge, PhD.

Všetky kurzy
Prihlásenie
Zabudnuté heslo

Zadajte e-mailovú adresu, s ktorou ste vytvárali účet. Budú Vám na ňu zasielané informácie k nastaveniu nového hesla.

Prihlásenie

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte sa

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#