Cognitive Performance in Late Adolescence and the Subsequent Risk of Subdural Hematoma: An Observational Study of a Prospective Nationwide Cohort
Background:
There are few identified risk factors for traumatic brain injuries such as subdural hematoma (SDH). The aim of the present study was to investigate whether low cognitive performance in young adulthood is associated with SDH later in life. A second aim was to investigate whether this risk factor was associated with education and physical fitness.
Methods and Findings:
Word recollection, logical, visuospatial, and technical performances were tested at a mean age of 18.5 years in a prospective nation-wide cohort of 440,742 men. An estimate of global intelligence was calculated from these four tests. Associations between cognitive performance, education, physical fitness, and SDH during follow-up were explored using Cox regression analyses. During a median follow-up of 35 years, 863 SDHs were diagnosed in the cohort. Low global intelligence was associated with an increased risk of SDH during follow-up (hazard ratio [HR]: 1.33, per standard deviation decrease, 95% CI = 1.25–1.43). Similar results were obtained for the other measures of cognitive performance (HR: 1.24–1.33, p<0.001 for all). In contrast, a high education (HR: 0.27, comparing more than 2 years of high school and 8 years of elementary school, 95% CI = 0.19–0.39), and a high level of physical fitness (HR: 0.76, per standard deviation increase, 95% CI = 0.70–0.83), was associated with a decreased risk of suffering from a SDH.
Conclusions:
The present findings suggest that reduced cognitive function in young adulthood is strongly associated with an increased risk of SDH later in life. In contrast, a higher level of education and a higher physical fitness were associated with a decreased risk of SDH.
: Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary
Published in the journal:
Cognitive Performance in Late Adolescence and the Subsequent Risk of Subdural Hematoma: An Observational Study of a Prospective Nationwide Cohort. PLoS Med 8(12): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001151
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001151
Summary
Background:
There are few identified risk factors for traumatic brain injuries such as subdural hematoma (SDH). The aim of the present study was to investigate whether low cognitive performance in young adulthood is associated with SDH later in life. A second aim was to investigate whether this risk factor was associated with education and physical fitness.
Methods and Findings:
Word recollection, logical, visuospatial, and technical performances were tested at a mean age of 18.5 years in a prospective nation-wide cohort of 440,742 men. An estimate of global intelligence was calculated from these four tests. Associations between cognitive performance, education, physical fitness, and SDH during follow-up were explored using Cox regression analyses. During a median follow-up of 35 years, 863 SDHs were diagnosed in the cohort. Low global intelligence was associated with an increased risk of SDH during follow-up (hazard ratio [HR]: 1.33, per standard deviation decrease, 95% CI = 1.25–1.43). Similar results were obtained for the other measures of cognitive performance (HR: 1.24–1.33, p<0.001 for all). In contrast, a high education (HR: 0.27, comparing more than 2 years of high school and 8 years of elementary school, 95% CI = 0.19–0.39), and a high level of physical fitness (HR: 0.76, per standard deviation increase, 95% CI = 0.70–0.83), was associated with a decreased risk of suffering from a SDH.
Conclusions:
The present findings suggest that reduced cognitive function in young adulthood is strongly associated with an increased risk of SDH later in life. In contrast, a higher level of education and a higher physical fitness were associated with a decreased risk of SDH.
: Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary
Introduction
The worldwide incidence of traumatic brain injury requiring medical attention or resulting in death was estimated at over 9.5 million in 1990 [1]. Subdural hematoma (SDH) is defined as a collection of blood between the brain and the dura mater usually from a laceration of bridge cortical veins associated with head injuries. SDH is the most common severe outcome of traumatic brain injury [2].
SDH can be divided into acute SDH or chronic SDH, which is diagnosed approximately 21 d after onset of bleeding [3],[4]. In a population-based study of traumatic brain injury in northern Sweden the proportion of isolated SDH in patients referred to a neurosurgery department was 51.2%, and of these 50.6% were diagnosed as chronic [2]. Symptoms of SDH include various aspects of impaired cognitive functioning. However, there have been very few studies that have actually investigated cognitive performance [5]–[7]. Cognitive impairment associated with SDH has most commonly been described as abnormal behavior or cognitive disturbances or deficits without any objective measurements [8]–[11]. The possibility of a latent development of dementia following head trauma has been suggested; the evidence for this, however, is inconclusive [12]. Another possibility is that low cognitive ability increases the risk of SDH and that the cognitive deficits seen after the occurrence of trauma were actually present before trauma. Low cognitive performance is associated with reduced perceptual speed [13] and impaired executive functions including poor judgment, planning, and risk behavior [14]–[16]. Thus, low cognitive functioning may be associated with several risk factors for accidents resulting in head injuries and SDHs [17],[18].
In the present study we hypothesized that low cognitive functioning in young adulthood may be associated with the later risk of a SDH. A second aim was to investigate whether this risk is associated with education and physical fitness.
Methods
Study Population
The cohort considered for inclusion in the present study consisted of all Swedish males that conscripted for compulsory military service between 1969 and 1978 (n = 457,302). These men represented approximately 97% of the male Swedish population born between 1951 and 1960 and the cohort was compiled from the Swedish Military Service Conscription Register (SMSCR). The background of the SMSCR has been described in detail previously [19]. Until recently, exemptions were granted only for incarcerated male subjects and those with documented severe chronic medical conditions or handicaps. For the present study men younger than 17 y at baseline or men, severely under or over weight (<40 or >170 kg), or of extreme stature (<140 or >215 cm), were excluded from further analysis (n = 9,545). We also excluded 6,975 additional men because of death or emigration before January, 1987 leaving 440,742 men for inclusion in the present study.
Ethics Statement
The present study was approved by the local ethics committee of Umeå University and by the National Board of Health and Welfare in Sweden.
Baseline Examination
All participants took part in a 2-d standardized intelligence and physical examination before conscription assignment to the Swedish Armed Forces at six regional conscription centers in Sweden. All conscripts underwent a medical examination by a physician and any disorders were diagnosed according to the Swedish version of International Classification of Disease (ICD), 8th edition. Weight, height, hearing, and vision were measured via standardized methods. Information on educational background was available for those who conscripted between 1972 and 1978 (n = 315,509), and was classified into four groups; 8 y of elementary school, 9 y of elementary school, 2 y of high school, and 3 y of high school or more. Physical capacity was assessed using an electrically braked bicycle ergometry test. In short, all participants with a normal resting electrocardiogram proceeded with a 5-min submaximal bicycling session at a work rate between 75 and 175 Watt (W) depending on body weight. Workload was then increased by 25 W/min until exhaustion occurred [20]. During the test, subjects were instructed to maintain pedal cadence between 60 and 70 revolutions per minute. Final work rate (physical fitness in W) was recorded. Data from the physical capacity test were available for 317,577 men who conscripted from 1972–1978.
Cognitive Tests
The following tests of cognitive performance have been described previously [21],[22]. The verbal test of synonyms measures the ability to correctly choose the synonym of a given word from four alternatives. The visuospatial geometric perception test assesses the capability to identify the correct three dimensional object form a series of two-dimensional drawings. The logical/inductive performance test measures the capacity to understand written instructions and apply them to a problem solving task. The theoretical/technical test assesses a component of general knowledge through a mathematical/physics problem. The maximum score for each of the first three tests was 40 points while the maximum score for the mathematical/physics problem was 52 points. The administered tests started with simple questions that gradually became more difficult. A measure of global intelligence was estimated from the four tests. Each test was of equal importance and weighted on the basis of the mean score and variation of each test. The result was expressed as a score with minimum of zero and a maximum of 40 points.
Diagnosis of SDH
Information on diagnosis of SDH between January, 1987 and December, 2009 was obtained through records from the National Hospital Discharge Register, covering all public inpatient care in Sweden, administered by the Center for Epidemiology at the National Board of Health and Welfare in Sweden. Diagnoses were recorded using the ICD version nine (1987–1997) and version ten (1998–2009). Deaths in the cohort occurring during the study period were collected through the National Cause of Death Register, administered by the Center for Epidemiology at the National Board of Health and Welfare in Sweden and emigration information was collected through the Statistics Sweden database.
Statistics
Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD) if not otherwise stated. Student's t tests for independent samples or Fischer's exact tests were performed to determine differences between subjects with and without SDH during follow-up. Associations between the explanatory variables at baseline and the later risk of SDH were tested using Cox proportional hazard models. Since age at baseline, differences over time as well as between the six test centers could introduce bias, age, year, and place of conscription were considered as confounders in all models. The independent effects of cognitive performance, education, and physical fitness were tested by including these variables in the same regression model. In this model all factors that were associated with the risk of a SDH according to Table 1 were considered as confounders. The joint effects of global intelligence and education for three groups of global intelligence and the four different groups of education were also assessed using Cox regression. The study endpoint for all Cox regression models was the date of an SDH, date of death, date of emigration, or December 31, 2009, whichever came first. The proportional hazard assumption was checked graphically using Kaplan-Meier curves. SPSS software for PCs (version 18.0; SPSS Inc.) was used for statistical analyses. All statistical tests were two-tailed. A p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
The present study followed 440,742 subjects from 18 y of age. During a median follow-up of 35 y (range 8–40), 863 subjects were diagnosed with a traumatic SDH at a median age of 46 y (range 28–59). Characteristics at baseline according to status during follow-up are shown in Table 1. Men diagnosed with a traumatic SDH were older, weighed less, and were shorter than those without SDH. In addition, men with a SDH diagnosis were less physically fit (p<0.001) and were more often diagnosed with substance abuse, personality disorders, and neurosis (p<0.01 for all measures). Men with SDH scored lower on all measures of cognitive performance at baseline (p<0.001 for all measures) and had a lower education (p<0.001) than the rest of the cohort. Using partial correlations and adjusting for age, year, and place of conscription, all measures of cognitive performance were associated with education (r = 0.24–0.42, p<0.001 for all correlations) and physical fitness (r = 0.12–0.19, p<0.001 for all correlations).
The most common cause of SDH was a fall in the same plane (Table 2). In total, falls explained about 58% of the SDHs occurring during follow-up. Other underlying causes included trauma during transportation and assault.
Using Cox regression (Model 1), after adjusting for age, year, and place of conscription, lower global cognitive performance (hazard ratio [HR]: 1.33 per SD decrease, 95% CI = 1.25–1.43 was associated with increased risk of SDH (Table 2). Similar results were found for the other measures of cognitive performance (Table 2). In contrast, a high education (HR: 0.27, comparing more than 2 y of high school and 8 y of elementary school 95% CI = 0.19–0.39), and a high level of physical fitness (HR: 0.76, per SD increase, 95% CI = 0.70–0.83) decreased the risk of SDH (Table 2). Adjusting the models including education or physical fitness also for global intelligence decreased the association slightly for education (HR: 0.40, 95% CI = 0.27–0.60) and for physical fitness (HR 0.81, per SD increase, 95% CI = 0.74–0.88). In the final regression model, global cognitive performance, education, physical fitness, all variables found to be associated with the risk of SDH according to Table 1, and also year and place of conscription, were used to explain the risk of SDH. In this model, global intelligence (HR 1.17, per SD decrease, 95% CI = 1.07–1.29), high education (HR: 0.46, 95% CI = 0.31–0.69, comparing >2 y of high school and 8 y of elementary school), and physical fitness (HR: 0.88, per SD increase, 95% CI = 0.79–0.97) were associated with SDH during follow-up.
Using Cox regression analysis, the independent effects of cognitive performance and education were further analyzed for low (≤15 points), medium (>15 points to ≤24 points), and high (>24 points) global intelligence (Table 3). Participants who were included in both the group with the highest education and the group with the highest global intelligence at baseline had 83% lower risk for SDH (HR: 0.17, 95% CI = 0.09–0.32) during follow-up, compared to those in the lowest group of both global intelligence and education.
The associations between cognitive performance, education, physical fitness, and the risk of SDH were also analyzed for each underlying cause of SDH. Overall, the strongest associations were found when the underlying cause of SDH was assault, while an underlying cause of trauma during transportation not involving motor vehicles was not significantly associated with the risk of SDH for any of the tests of cognitive performance, education, or physical fitness (Table 2).
The associations between cognitive performance and the risk of SDH were further analyzed for quintiles of each measure, with the highest quintile as reference (Table 4). When comparing the highest to lowest quintiles of cognitive performance, global intelligence (HR: 2.31, 95% CI = 1.86–2.88), logical performance (HR: 2.14, 95% CI = 1.71–2.67), word recollection (HR: 1.89, 95% CI = 1.53–2.35), visuospatial performance (HR: 1.97, 95% CI = 1.58–2.47), and technical performance (HR: 1.85, 95% CI = 1.49–2.30) were found to be significantly associated with the risk of SDH after adjustment for age, place, and year of conscription. The cumulative incidence of SDH for five different groups of global intelligence is shown in Figure 1. Men in the lowest group of global intelligence (≤15 points) reached a cumulative incidence of 130/100,000 subjects with an SDH diagnosis 13 y earlier than those with a global intelligence of ≥25 points. Assuming a linear relationship, the cumulative incidence increased by 4.8 SDHs/year (95% CI = 4.6–4.9) in the group with highest cognitive performance compared to 9.8 SDHs/year (95% CI = 9.5–10.1) in the group with lowest cognitive performance.
Discussion
The results of the present study indicate that low cognitive performance in young adulthood is associated with an increased risk of sustaining a traumatic SDH later in life. Moreover, general education and objective measures of physical fitness were associated both with cognitive performance in young adulthood and the risk of SDH later in life. In a final model, a high compared to a low education and physical fitness decreased the risk of a SDH by 54% and 12%, respectively. We suggest that such estimated effects may be important on a public health level if the results could be replicated in other epidemiological settings.
The clinical importance of cognitive performance as a risk factor for the future risk of SDH may be illustrated in Figure 1. The cumulative incidence of SDH was followed in five different groups with respect to global intelligence. In our population of men, 12% had an estimated global intelligence of at most 15 points, and 24% of the men had a score of at least 25 points. When comparing these groups, men in the group with the lower score reached a specified cumulative incidence of SDHs 13 y earlier. Thus, these results suggest that a high cognitive functioning is associated with a delayed occurrence of SDH by more than a decade.
SDHs have many negative consequences including pain, debility, and reduced self-dependency [23],[24], and therefore, manipulation of environmental factors that may affect cognitive performance could prove to be beneficial to reduce the risk of SDHs. In the present study, we especially evaluated the importance of education and objective measures of physical fitness. Education and physical fitness were strongly associated with all measures of cognitive performance at baseline. Education and physical fitness were also independently associated with the risk of SDH during follow-up. With respect to education, men with at least 3 y of high school had a 73% decreased risk of suffering SDH during follow-up, compared to those with 8 y of elementary school. Moreover, most of the estimated effects of education on the risk of SDH were independent of cognitive performance. In support of this result, higher education is associated with more desk jobs, which may be associated with fewer environmental hazards than manual labor. A high physical fitness at baseline was also associated with a decreased risk of SDH. Thus, every SD increase in physical fitness decreased the risk of SDH by 24% during follow-up. To better evaluate the importance of education and physical fitness, both these factors were included in the final regression model to explain the risk of SDH, adjusting for global cognitive performance and all significant confounders at baseline. At least 3 y of high school was then found to reduce the risk of SDH by 54%, and a higher physical fitness was found to be associated with a 12%. decreased risk. Confirmation of these findings in other large cohorts would be useful to validate these associations. An exploration of the mechanistic basis for these associations might allow the construction of public health interventions aimed at reducing the population incidence of SDH, define high risk groups in whom these interventions might be most usefully trialed, and identify intermediate endpoints that might provide further mechanistic explanation of benefit (if detected). For example, increasing the proportion of young males receiving secondary school education may result in reduced alcohol and/or substance abuse, less job-related environmental hazards, and could, by this effect, reduce the risk of head injury.
A few studies have investigated cognitive performance in relation to chronic SDH and these suggest that reduced cognitive function is present after acquired SDH [5]–[7]. Also occurrence of mild traumatic head injury, usually referred to as concussion, has been associated with lower cognitive performance [25],[26]. However, there are no previous data linking SDHs or concussions to premorbid cognitive performance. Our data suggest that the level of cognitive performance prior to SDH may in part be responsible for cognitive functioning after SDH acquisition. The results from the present study could also have implications for the rehabilitation process following SDH. A low premorbid cognitive performance could affect the outcome of SDH [27]. The predominantly metabolic pathophysiology during the initial period after a traumatic brain injury, may be more difficult for individuals with low cognitive capacity to overcome resulting in an impeded degree of recovery and adaptation [28],[29].
Given that the studied cohort consisted of only men inferences from these results to women should be made with caution. We cannot exclude that there are confounders not accounted for in the present study that could influence the associations found. Low socio-economic class would be one such factor that probably influences the education level of the participants in present study. Education at baseline was only available for the 315,509 men that conscripted from 1972–1978. In addition, the present study was observational and thus inferences about causality should also be made with great caution. However, some of the findings in the present study may suggest a cause–effect relationship. The associations found between cognition, education, physical fitness, and the risk of SDH were independent of medical conditions and other confounders accounted for at baseline. There was also a dose-dependent relationship between cognitive performance and the risk of SDH. Furthermore, the associations between the main independent variables (cognitive performance, education, and physical fitness) and the risk of SDH showed a consistent pattern for the different underlying causes of SDH. Thus, the strongest association for any measure of cognitive performance, education, physical fitness, and SDH was found when the underlying cause was assault. With respect to this result it is of interest to note that reduced cognitive performance is associated with problem behavior and impaired judgment [14],[15].
In summary, in a large population-based analysis, low cognitive performance at 18 y of age was strongly associated with traumatic SDH during a median of 35 y of follow-up. In contrast, a high education and physical fitness was associated with a decreased risk of traumatic SDH independent of the level of cognitive performance. In addition, the current results suggest that the lower cognitive functioning seen after traumatic brain injury may be influenced by premorbid cognitive performance.
Zdroje
1. MurrayCLopezA 1996 Global health statistics: a compendium of incidence, prevalence and mortality estimates for over 200 conditions Boston The Harvard School of Public Health on behalf of the World Health Organization and the World Bank
2. JacobssonLJWesterbergMLexellJ 2007 Demographics, injury characteristics and outcome of traumatic brain injuries in northern Sweden. Acta Neurol Scand 116 300 306
3. ScottiGTerbruggeKMelanconDBelangerG 1977 Evaluation of the age of subdural hematomas by computerized tomography. J Neurosurg 47 311 315
4. NakaguchiHTanishimaTYoshimasuN 2001 Factors in the natural history of chronic subdural hematomas that influence their postoperative recurrence. J Neurosurg 95 256 262
5. TsuboiKMakiYNoseTMatsukiT 1984 [Psychiatric symptoms of patients with chronic subdural hematoma]. No Shinkei Geka 12 275 279
6. OyamaHUedaMInoueSIkedaAShibuyaM 1998 [Improvement of cognition after trepanation for the chronic subdural hematoma]. No To Shinkei 50 249 252
7. MachuldaMMHautMW 2000 Clinical features of chronic subdural hematoma: neuropsychiatric and neuropsychologic changes in patients with chronic subdural hematoma. Neurosurg Clin N Am 11 473 477
8. MirandaLBBraxtonEHobbsJQuigleyMR 2011 Chronic subdural hematoma in the elderly: not a benign disease. J Neurosurg 114 72 76
9. Gelabert-GonzalezMIglesias-PaisMGarcia-AllutAMartinez-RumboR 2005 Chronic subdural haematoma: surgical treatment and outcome in 1000 cases. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 107 223 229
10. CameronMM 1978 Chronic subdural haematoma: a review of 114 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 41 834 839
11. ParyRTobiasCRLippmannS 1990 Dementia: what to do. South Med J 83 1182 1189
12. JellingerKA 2004 Head injury and dementia. Curr Opin Neurol 17 719 723
13. BackmanLLindenbergerULiSCNybergL 2010 Linking cognitive aging to alterations in dopamine neurotransmitter functioning: recent data and future avenues. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34 670 677
14. DenklaM 1996 Research on executive function in a neurodevelopmental context: application of clinical measures. Dev Neuropsychology 12 5 15
15. RoyallDRLauterbachECCummingsJLReeveARummansTA 2002 Executive control function: a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 14 377 405
16. SternY 2009 Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47 2015 2028
17. SmithDKirkhamR 1982 Relationship between intelligence and driving record. Accid Anal Prev 14 439 442
18. LawlorDAClarkHLeonDA 2007 Associations between childhood intelligence and hospital admissions for unintentional injuries in adulthood: the Aberdeen Children of the 1950 s cohort study. Am J Public Health 97 291 297
19. AndreassonSAllebeckPRomelsjoA 1988 Alcohol and mortality among young men: longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 296 1021 1025
20. AndersenLB 1995 A maximal cycle exercise protocol to predict maximal oxygen uptake. Scand J Med Sci Sports 5 143 146
21. CarlstedtBMårdbergB 1993 Construct validity of the Swedish Enlistment Battery. Scand J Psychol 34 353 362
22. CarlstedtB 2000 Cognitive abilities - aspects of stucture, process and measurement Gothenburg University of Gothenburg
23. KasteMWaltimoOHeiskanenO 1979 Chronic bilateral subdural haematoma in adults. Acta neurochir 48 231 236
24. DronfieldMWMeadGMLangmanMJ 1977 Survival and death from subdural haematoma on medical wards. Postgrad Med J 53 57 60
25. De MonteVEGeffenGMMassavelliBM 2006 The effects of post-traumatic amnesia on information processing following mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 20 1345 1354
26. VanderploegRDCurtissGBelangerHG 2005 Long-term neuropsychological outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 11 228 236
27. RyanTVSautterSWCappsCFMeneeseWBarthJT 1992 Utilizing neuropsychological measures to predict vocational outcome in a head trauma population. Brain Inj 6 175 182
28. StahlNSchalenWUngerstedtUNordstromCH 2003 Bedside biochemical monitoring of the penumbra zone surrounding an evacuated acute subdural haematoma. Acta Neurol Scand 108 211 215
29. KushiHMoriyaTSaitoTKinoshitaKShibuyaT 1999 Importance of metabolic monitoring systems as an early prognostic indicator in severe head injured patients. Acta neurochir Suppl 75 67 68
Štítky
Interné lekárstvoČlánok vyšiel v časopise
PLOS Medicine
2011 Číslo 12
- Statiny indukovaná myopatie: Jak na diferenciální diagnostiku?
- MUDr. Dana Vondráčková: Hepatopatie sú pri liečbe metamizolom väčším strašiakom ako agranulocytóza
- Vztah mezi statiny a rizikem vzniku nádorových onemocnění − metaanalýza
- Nech brouka žít… Ať žije astma!
- Parazitičtí červi v terapii Crohnovy choroby a dalších zánětlivých autoimunitních onemocnění
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
- Violent Crime, Epilepsy, and Traumatic Brain Injury
- Rotating Night Shift Work and Risk of Type 2 Diabetes: Two Prospective Cohort Studies in Women
- Surgery Versus Epilation for the Treatment of Minor Trichiasis in Ethiopia: A Randomised Controlled Noninferiority Trial
- Worldwide Incidence of Malaria in 2009: Estimates, Time Trends, and a Critique of Methods