#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Psychological, social, and welfare interventions for torture survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials


Authors: Aseel Hamid aff001;  Nimisha Patel aff002;  Amanda C. de C. Williams aff001
Authors place of work: Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom aff001;  Clinical Psychology, School of Psychology, University of East London, London, United Kingdom aff002;  International Centre for Health & Human Rights, London, United Kingdom aff003
Published in the journal: Psychological, social, and welfare interventions for torture survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. PLoS Med 16(9): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002919
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002919

Summary

Background

Torture and other forms of ill treatment have been reported in at least 141 countries, exposing a global crisis. Survivors face multiple physical, psychological, and social difficulties. Psychological consequences for survivors are varied, and evidence on treatment is mixed. We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to estimate the benefits and harms of psychological, social, and welfare interventions for torture survivors.

Methods and findings

We updated a 2014 review with published randomised controlled trials (RCTs) for adult survivors of torture comparing any psychological, social, or welfare intervention against treatment as usual or active control from 1 January 2014 through 22 June 2019. Primary outcome was post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms or caseness, and secondary outcomes were depression symptoms, functioning, quality of life, and adverse effects, after treatment and at follow-up of at least 3 months. Standardised mean differences (SMDs) and odds ratios were estimated using meta-analysis with random effects. The Cochrane tool was used to derive risk of bias. Fifteen RCTs were included, with data from 1,373 participants (589 females and 784 males) in 10 countries (7 trials in Europe, 5 in Asia, and 3 in Africa). No trials of social or welfare interventions were found. Compared to mostly inactive (waiting list) controls, psychological interventions reduced PTSD symptoms by the end of treatment (SMD −0.31, 95% confidence interval [CI] −0.52 to −0.09, p = 0.005), but PTSD symptoms at follow-up were not significantly reduced (SMD −0.34, 95% CI −0.74 to 0.06, p = 0.09). No significant improvement was found for PTSD caseness at the end of treatment, and there was possible worsening at follow-up from one study (n = 28). Interventions showed no benefits for depression symptoms at end of treatment (SMD −0.23, 95% CI −0.50 to 0.03, p = 0.09) or follow-up (SMD −0.23, 95% CI −0.70 to 0.24, p = 0.34). A significant improvement in functioning for psychological interventions compared to control was found at end of treatment (SMD −0.38, 95% CI −0.58 to −0.18, p = 0.0002) but not at follow-up from only one study. No significant improvement emerged for quality of life at end of treatment (SMD 0.38, 95% CI −0.28 to 1.05, p = 0.26) with no data available at follow-up. The main study limitations were the difficulty in this field of being certain of capturing all eligible studies, the lack of modelling of maintenance of treatment gains, and the low precision of most SMDs making findings liable to change with the addition of further studies as they are published.

Conclusions

Our findings show evidence that psychological interventions improve PTSD symptoms and functioning at the end of treatment, but it is unknown whether this is maintained at follow-up, with a possible worsening of PTSD caseness at follow-up from one study. Further interventions in this population should address broader psychological needs beyond PTSD while taking into account the effect of multiple daily stressors. Additional studies, including social and welfare interventions, will improve precision of estimates of effect, particularly over the longer term.

Keywords:

Quality of life – Systematic reviews – Culture – depression – Post-traumatic stress disorder – Social welfare – Welfare (social security)

Introduction

Despite 156 countries having signed the United Nations Convention Against Torture and Other Cruel, Inhuman or Degrading Treatment and Punishment [1], torture is widespread, and Amnesty International has documented torture and other forms of ill treatment in 141 countries in 2014 [2]. Long-standing and ongoing armed conflict has likely led to the increased use of torture since. Worldwide, 352,000 fatalities resulting from organised violence were identified between 2014 and 2016 alone [3]. The prevalence of torture and resulting fatalities are likely higher but difficult to estimate given that perpetrators often obscure the use of torture, and there are multiple barriers to disclosure for survivors.

Torture has psychological, physical, social, and spiritual impacts that interact in diverse ways. Psychological effects are well documented; predominantly post-traumatic stress, depression, anxiety, and phobias [4,5]. Physical effects are also diverse (for reviews, see [6,7]). In addition, torture survivors’ disrupted lives can bring social and financial problems that contribute to and maintain psychological distress, whether as a refugee or in the country of origin [5,8,9].

Torture often occurs against a backdrop of national and international power imbalances, war, civil unrest, and the destruction or erosion of medical and other welfare services. Arguably, treatment needs to incorporate wider conceptualisations of damage and distress than are represented in standard Western psychological treatments for psychological trauma [10,11]. A review conducted in 2011 describes a limited range of interventions for torture survivors, tested in studies with significant limitations such as small sample sizes and unvalidated outcomes [6]. Given the scant literature, greater understanding of what works in treatment and rehabilitation for torture survivors is crucial in order to obtain maximum benefits from scarce resources.

A Cochrane systematic review and meta-analysis [12] aimed to summarise psychological, social, and welfare interventions for torture survivors but found eligible studies only of psychological treatment. The 9 randomised controlled trials (RCTs) included provided data on 507 adults with no immediate benefits for psychological therapy for psychological distress (as measured by depression symptoms), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, PTSD caseness, or quality of life. At follow-up, 4 studies with 86 participants showed moderate effect sizes in reducing psychological distress and PTSD symptoms. Conclusions were tentative, given the low quality of evidence, with underpowered studies and outcomes assessed in nonstandard ways, and no study assessed participation in community life or social and family relationships.

More recently, a meta-analysis of 18 pre-post studies of interventions for survivors of mass violence in low- and middle-income countries showed a large improvement in PTSD and depression across treatment [13] but smaller effects from controlled studies. Another recent review [14] concluded that cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) interventions produced the best treatment outcome for PTSD and/or depression. However, both reviews recruited more widely than torture survivors. No recent systematic reviews or meta-analyses have focused on interventions for torture survivors. We conducted this systematic review and meta-analysis to assess the reported benefits or adverse outcomes in the domains of PTSD symptoms, PTSD caseness, psychological distress, functioning, and quality of life for psychological, social, and welfare interventions for torture survivors.

Methods

Search strategy and selection criteria

A systematic review was performed using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement [15], which is available in S1 PRISMA Checklist. To be included, studies had to be RCTs or quasi-RCTs of psychological, social, or welfare interventions for survivors of torture against any active or inactive comparison condition; the same criteria were used as in the previous review [12], and the full protocol is provided in S1 Text. Quasi-RCTs, in which the method of allocation is known but not strictly random—such as the use of alternation, date of birth, and medical record number [16]—were included considering the difficulties of conducting RCTs in this population.

We extracted RCTs from searches of PsycINFO, MEDLINE, EMBASE, Web of Science, the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, the WHO International Clinical Trials Registry Platform, Clinical Trials.Gov, PTSDpubs, and the online library of Danish Institute Against Torture (DIGNITY) databases from 1 January 2014 (1 January 2013 in the case of Web of Science, the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature, and PTSDpubs) through 22 June 2019 using key search terms including combinations of “torture,” “randomised,” “trial,” and “intervention” with Boolean operators (S1 Text). There was no language restriction. We also searched reference lists of torture-specific reviews published in or after January 2014 and those emerging from the final set of included studies. We contacted corresponding authors when full texts were unavailable.

Data extraction

We initially screened titles and abstracts against the inclusion criteria, with the aim of identifying potentially eligible studies for which the full paper was obtained. One author (AH) initially screened titles and abstracts to select full papers; another author (AW) checked a subsample of the excluded papers and agreed with all exclusions. Full papers were screened and selected for inclusion by 2 authors independently and agreed upon after discussion (AH and AW).

Descriptive data, including participant characteristics, treatment mode, and setting, were collected. The primary area of interest for this review was outcomes in the domains of PTSD symptoms and caseness, psychological distress, functioning, and quality of life. PTSD symptoms were defined as the primary outcome given that the majority of identified reviews measured this. Psychological distress was measured as a secondary outcome, in the form of depression symptoms. Depression was chosen to define psychological distress because it is more distinct from PTSD than alternative scaled constructs of psychological distress, particularly anxiety. As in Patel and colleagues’ review [12], functioning was measured by engagement in education, training, work, or community activity, and quality of life was defined as a change (positive or negative) in quality of life or well-being as measured by global satisfaction with life and extent of disability.

Statistical analyses

Studies in which a psychological, social, or welfare intervention was an active treatment of primary interest were investigated. When studies included more than one arm within a trial, it was decided that—where both arms represented the same content of intervention—data from those arms were combined. The respective control arms associated with these intervention arms were also combined, given that the main area of interest of this research is the impact of intervention relative to control. In studies in which both adjusted and unadjusted treatment effects for specific covariates were reported, the adjusted treatment effects were used.

Due to varying data collection and reporting methods, this review included both continuous and dichotomous scales. Meta-analyses were conducted using Review Manager (RevMan version 5.3) software [16]. It was anticipated that there would be considerable heterogeneity in the data, measured as I2, so a random-effects model was applied.

For continuous scales, treatment effects were estimated using standardised mean differences (SMDs). This requires the extraction of mean scores, standard deviations, and sample sizes for each arm. When standard deviations required for the analyses were not available, they were calculated from confidence intervals (CIs), as suggested in the Cochrane handbook [16]. For dichotomous data, treatment effects were estimated using odds ratios by extracting the number of events and sample sizes. All analyses were conducted as planned.

The newly included studies were added to the 9 previous studies in each analysis. Analyses were run for end of treatment and follow-up when available. End of treatment was defined as data collected within 3 months or less from the end of treatment; follow-up was defined as more than 3 months after the end of treatment.

Quality of studies

The risks of bias were assessed using the Cochrane guidance [16]. Each study was classified for each of the categories into either low risk, high risk or unclear risk, with justifications. This quality assessment was completed by 2 authors independently (AH and AW), and disagreements were resolved by reference to the data in question. We related the risk of bias categories to the interpretation of effect sizes for the outcomes of studies.

Results

From an initial screen of 1,805 abstracts and titles, 6 RCTs since 2014 met our inclusion criteria [1722] and were combined with the 9 RCTs identified in the previous meta-analysis (Fig 1) [2331]. The characteristics of the 15 included studies are summarised in S1 Table. All eligible studies were of psychological interventions. Trials included 1,373 participants at the end of treatment (mean per study = 92) of the 1,585 that started treatment; a mean study completion rate of 86.6% with a range from 50% to 100%. Studies included 589 females and 784 males. Seven trials were conducted in Europe, 5 in Asia, and 3 in Africa. The most commonly used intervention was narrative exposure therapy (4 studies) or testimony therapy (3 studies), both of which draw on creating a testimony of traumatic events. Of the 6 new studies, all provided analysable data after calculating the standard deviation from CIs or standard errors. When neither CIs nor mean scores were available [14,21], the author was contacted, and the mean scores and standard deviations were obtained.

Fig. 1. Study flow diagram.
Study flow diagram.

Quality of studies

According to Cochrane risk of bias assessment [16], one study had a high risk of bias in random sequence generation, 2 had a high risk of bias in allocation concealment, all 15 had a high risk of performance bias (inevitable in psychological treatment trials), 2 had a high risk of detection bias, 6 had a high risk of attrition bias, and no studies had a high risk of reporting bias. Therapist allegiance, treatment fidelity, therapist qualifications, and other biases were also included. Four studies had a high risk of bias due to therapist allegiance, 2 had a high risk of bias due to therapist fidelity, and 2 had a high risk of bias due to therapist qualifications (Fig 2). Other biases included varying content and length of treatment as judged by therapist according to need, as well as the absence of protocol for adaptation and translation of measures. A full breakdown of the risk of bias in each study is available in S1 Table.

Fig. 2. Risk of bias.
Risk of bias.

PTSD symptoms

Twelve trials, with a total of 1,086 participants, reported data for PTSD symptoms no more than 3 months after the end of treatment [1721,2431], using several scales but all based on a similar formulation of PTSD. They were analysed for the effect of psychological intervention on PTSD at end of treatment using SMDs (Fig 3). There was a small to moderate reduction in PTSD symptomatology at the end of treatment (SMD −0.31, 95% CI −0.52 to −0.09, z = 2.79, p = 0.005). Between-study heterogeneity, I2, was 55% (95% CI 0.38–0.68), indicating substantial heterogeneity [16]. The confidence in these results is limited overall, as unblinding of assessors may have contributed to detection bias in all but one study [30].

Fig. 3. Effect of intervention on PTSD symptoms at end of treatment and follow-up.
Effect of intervention on PTSD symptoms at end of treatment and follow-up.
PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

Seven trials, with 569 participants, reported data for PTSD symptoms more than 3 months after the end of treatment [19,2124,26,27]. All used the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire (HTQ) to measure symptoms with the exception of Esala and Taing [19], who used the PTSD Checklist for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). They were analysed for the effect of psychological intervention on PTSD at follow-up using SMDs (Fig 3).

There was no difference between the intervention group and the control group (SMD −0.34, 95% CI −0.74 to 0.06, z = 1.68, p = 0.09) in PTSD symptoms at follow-up. Given the large CI, the precision of estimate was low, and all but one study [22] appeared to be underpowered. Heterogeneity was substantial at 66% (95% CI 0.49–0.77).

PTSD caseness

Four trials with 82 total participants, classifying participants using caseness as meeting criteria for PTSD no more than 3 months after the end of intervention [21,23,24,30], were analysed for the effect of psychological intervention on PTSD caseness at end of treatment (Fig 4). There was no overall benefit, with an odds ratio of 0.44 (95% CI 0.14–1.31, z = 1.48, p = 0.14). A heterogeneity of I2 = 0% was noted for this comparison (95% CI 0–0.61), and a number of sources of bias in methodology were observed.

Fig. 4. Effect of intervention on PTSD caseness at end of treatment.
Effect of intervention on PTSD caseness at end of treatment.
M-H, Mantel-Haenszel; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

Only one trial compared PTSD caseness in intervention and control groups, at 6-month follow-up for 28 participants [21]. Caseness was significantly higher at 6-month follow-up in the intervention group compared with the control group, with an odds ratio of 7.58 (95% CI 1.2–48, z = 2.15, p = 0.03).

Psychological distress

Ten trials reported data for psychological distress, measured as depression, no more than 3 months after the end of treatment, with 988 participants [1722,24,25,27,30]. They were analysed for the effect of psychological intervention on psychological distress at the end of treatment (Fig 5). There was no benefit of treatment over control (SMD −0.23, 95% CI −0.50 to 0.03, z = 1.71, p = 0.09) with a substantial heterogeneity of I2 = 68% (95% CI 0.56–0.77).

Fig. 5. Effect of intervention on psychological distress at end of treatment and follow-up.
Effect of intervention on psychological distress at end of treatment and follow-up.

Seven trials reported data for psychological distress, measured as depression using the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 (HSCL-25), more than 3 months after the end of treatment, with a total of 569 participants [19,2124,26,27]. They were analysed for the effect of psychological intervention on psychological distress at follow-up using SMDs (Fig 5). There was no benefit of treatment over control for psychological distress at follow-up (SMD −0.23, 95% CI −0.70 to 0.24, z = 0.96, p = 0.34), and heterogeneity was considerable (I2 = 76%, 95% CI 0.65–0.80).

Functioning

Three trials reported data for functioning at the end of treatment, for 584 participants [17,18,21], and were analysed for the effect of psychological intervention on functioning at the end of treatment (Fig 6). There was a moderate benefit of intervention over control for functioning (SMD −0.38, 95% CI −0.58 to −0.18, z = 3.72, p = 0.0002). A heterogeneity of I2 = 15% was observed (95% CI 0–0.73).

Fig. 6. Effect of intervention on functioning at end of treatment.
Effect of intervention on functioning at end of treatment.

Only one study (28 participants) provided analysable data showing effects at 6-month follow-up [21] and no statistically significant benefits for treatment over control (SMD 0.63, 95% CI −0.13 to 1.40, z = 1.62, p = −0.11).

Quality of life

Two trials [20,30], with 36 participants, assessed quality of life after treatment. Their scales were constructed with opposite direction for improvement; the trial by Puvimanasinghe and Price [20] was reversed so that a positive effect size represented improvement. There was no effect of intervention over control on quality of life (SMD 0.38, 95% CI −0.28 to 1.05, z = 1.14, p = 0.26) with a low precision of estimate. No study assessed quality of life at follow-up.

Adverse events and dropout

Two studies reported on adverse effects of treatment. Weiss and colleagues [22] reported that one participant attempted suicide after the first therapy session. The authors related this to the participant being related to the therapist and the therapist failing to notify the supervisor due to stigma concerns in the family. Another participant was hospitalised with severe depression and received therapy in the hospital but did not return to the study, and one participant died of a heart attack with no apparent relationship to participation in the study. In Wang and colleagues’ [21] study, the intervention group increased in PTSD caseness over follow-up, a statistically significant finding, but the authors were not able to explain this result.

All but 2 trials [23,29] reported dropout during treatment. Of these, 4 reported greater than 20% dropout in the intervention arm [19,24,27,30], and one trial reported a 28% exclusion of participants overall, with no further detail given [28]. Four studies provided detailed reasons for dropout [18,24,27,30].

Clinical meaning of changes

Calculation of the SMD assumes that differences in standard deviations among studies reflect differences in assessment scales and not real differences in variability among study populations [16]. We chose Wang and colleagues’ study [21] to calculate differences in PTSD symptoms using the HTQ, and in psychological distress (depression) using the HSCL-25. The HTQ uses a 4-point severity response scale. Respondents endorse how much each symptom has bothered them in the past week, as follows: not at all (1), a little bit (2), quite a bit (3), or extremely (4). The total score is the mean of item scores, with 2.5 suggested as the clinical cut-off score, above which a respondent has a high likelihood of PTSD [32]. The small to moderate effect size in reduction of PTSD symptoms for intervention over control represented reduction of the mean pretreatment HTQ score of 2.49 to 2.37 post treatment. That is, participants fell slightly below clinical cut-off both before and after treatment, so the clinical significance of this change is negligible. The HSCL-25 assessed depression, with 1.75 suggested as the clinical cut-off score, with high scores indicating depression. Again relating these scores to the study by Wang and colleagues [21], mean scores at pretreatment assessment (3.02), post-treatment assessment (2.77), and follow-up (2.55) all fell within the clinical range for depression.

Discussion

This systematic review and meta-analysis of 15 studies of interventions for torture survivors included 1,373 participants from 10 countries. Six of the 15 studies were published since the previous review, but the sample size increased 3-fold. The range of treatments was somewhat wider, but treatments were still most often compared with inactive controls rather than with other treatment. The problems of torture survivors were largely conceptualised in terms of PTSD symptoms that constituted the focus of treatment and, often, the primary outcome. Meta-analysis demonstrated few benefits of treatment: a statistically significant but clinically small decrease in PTSD symptoms at the end of treatment—from varied psychological interventions compared to mostly inactive controls—not found at follow-up. Other outcomes—PTSD caseness, psychological distress, usually depression and often of clinical severity—were not significantly different either at the end of treatment or at follow-up, with the exception of a worsening of PTSD caseness at follow-up, a poorer outcome than in the previous review [12] and clinically very disappointing. Few studies assessed functioning or quality of life, so results must be interpreted with caution, but they showed no improvement in quality of life and only in functioning, at the end of treatment but not at follow-up.

Outcomes representing broader health and participation in society were neglected, as was the context of social, economic, and political uncertainties survivors face; threats to civil and legal status, accommodation, safety, connections with family and friends, and other assaults on well-being [8,33,34]. Because refugees have a high rate of life events that can facilitate or undermine treatment gains, it would be helpful for studies to monitor these changes across the timescale of treatment and follow-up [35]. It was disappointing to find these shortcomings persisting despite comment in our previous review [12] and in others [36,37].

Although it should be interpreted with caution, the finding of worsening at follow-up in the study by Wang and colleagues [21], using CBT with prolonged exposure, should alert researchers to the importance of studying long-term outcomes and the potentially harmful effects of psychological interventions and other contextual factors post treatment. Furthermore, 4 out of the 15 trials reported over 20% dropout in the intervention arm. It is possible that this is a function of greater social instability of the participant population and understandable preoccupation with meeting basic human needs and rights. However, more investigation of treatment expectations and acceptability is required. Conducting and analysing follow-up interviews, using nonaligned and nonbiased interviewers, would lead towards better understanding of what may work and for whom.

Other reviews of psychological treatments for torture survivors [36,38] or for traumatised refugees [39] have produced more optimistic accounts of benefits of therapy, although they raise similar concerns regarding methodology and cultural appropriateness of interventions. By contrast, Salo and Bray [37] reviewed interventions in relation to what they described (drawing on Bronfenbrenner [40]) as the ‘ecological’ needs of torture survivors: microsystem life domain, such as family, social, legal, and occupational domains; macrosystem domain, mainly consisting of cultural and language features of the trials; and the chronosystem domain, represented in time of follow-up assessments. They found relatively scant recognition of needs in any of these areas, either in assessment or intervention. This appears to be a very promising framework for reconsidering therapeutic interventions in the field.

Methodological quality of the included studies was largely similar to that in our previous review. Apart from the absence of blinding of therapists or patients to treatment allocation, rarely possible in trials of psychological treatment, bias arose mainly from incomplete reporting of outcomes, dropping noncompleters from outcome analysis, and uncertainty about whether the intended treatment had been delivered as designed, mainly because of lack of therapist qualifications to deliver it. Whether training volunteer therapists, with no existing clinical competences, in the specific therapeutic techniques for the trial is adequate to produce treatment fidelity is an open question and should be addressed within trials. The same comment applies to cultural adaptation of treatment that originated in Western healthcare. Studies gave little detail of what it was they meant by ‘cultural adaptation’, beyond translation of outcome scales and treatment materials, but effective cultural adaptation involves extensive work between people from all the main cultures represented in a study, who understand the context and content of treatment. Similar methods are required for true validation of translated scales in the languages of the cultures in which they will be used [41]. Even when these procedures are followed, it is by no means clear how a treatment is established as culturally adapted beyond the claims of its authors.

The review has some limitations that potentially affect conclusions. Our search could have been widened by including the grey literature, but a zero yield from around 1,500 chapters, reports, and other articles accessed for the previous review decided us against it. It is possible that in the grey literature, or even in the peer-reviewed literature, our relatively broad search nevertheless missed a trial labelled in a way we did not anticipate, since the nomenclature is not well standardised. While we did not exclude studies in other languages, the majority of the databases searched have shown varied and incomplete coverage of non-English material [42] particularly from low- and middle-income countries [43], indicating a potential database coverage bias. A possible further analysis would have been to fit a model to all effect sizes of each outcome, including time (end of treatment versus follow-up) as a moderator; because we did not, we cannot draw conclusions about maintenance of treatment gains at follow-up. We interpreted our findings according to dichotomous notions of statistical significance and recognise that some overall effect sizes could change (for better or worse) with the addition of one or more studies.

Heterogeneity among studies was substantial and arose from multiple sources: participants, therapists, therapeutic methods, outcomes, delivery, and setting. This produced generally high levels of between-study heterogeneity (I2) that made estimates of effect sensitive to inclusion or exclusion of single studies. Given the weakness and lack of precision of the I2 statistical test [44], we also calculated the CIs as suggested by Higgins and Thompson [45]. While CIs were generally narrow in cases of high heterogeneity, where low heterogeneity was indicated, I2 = 0% for PTSD caseness at end of treatment and I2 = 15% for functioning at end of treatment, wide CIs were produced ranging from 0% to 61% and 73%, respectively, indicating caution in inferring heterogeneity in these cases. We did not anticipate having the power available for subanalyses, but these could be planned in a further update, to investigate each source of heterogeneity. Although widening our scope to refugee studies would have included some family and community interventions, heterogeneity would likely have been even greater, exacerbating problems of interpretation.

Given the complexity of torture survivors’ needs and the obstacles they face in reconstructing a meaningful life, the emphasis of interventions on symptoms of PTSD is strikingly narrow, unless reducing or resolving these symptoms is seen as a priority or as the key to other improvements; none of the studies asserted this. It is not even clear that basic security and financial needs are addressed before offering specialised psychological interventions [46]. Thus, integration of interventions addressing the needs and priorities of torture survivors (not assessed or stated) seemed largely lacking. Recruitment into the trial was assumed to mean that survivors’ PTSD symptoms were their priority as the target of intervention, though the fact that 4 out of 15 included studies reported greater than 20% dropout in the intervention arm raises questions about the relevance and appropriateness of the interventions for survivors. Furthermore, the development of interventions in terms of cultural and language appropriateness may require more fundamental exploration and questioning of Western models of psychological problems and treatment than was evident in these trials. Recent models of collaborative care [47] go some way towards this but still fall short of the ecological scope described by Salo and Bray [37]. Last, where resources are scarce and far outstripped by needs, as in many low- and middle-income countries, the model of training local volunteers or healthcare staff in Western methods of intervention delivered mostly as individual therapy may mean that interventions are more culturally embedded (depending on what the trainers or study researchers allow by way of adaptation). This, however, needs empirical support, as well as an assessment of the potential harmful impacts on the volunteer therapists and on (other) survivors they work with.

Perhaps it is the limitation of this review to RCTs that means that the newer trials largely resembled the older ones, except in combining a wider range of interventions; there was little evidence of more collaborative and integrated interventions such as those developing for refugee populations [48] or envisaged in a social-ecological framework [37]. It might be that single case methods [49] are more applicable to assessing psychological, social, welfare, and other interventions for the complex and diverse needs of many torture survivors, for whom distress stems not only from the violent and traumatic experiences endured but also from current social, material, and legal conditions [34].

Evaluation of interventions needs to match this breadth of difficulties and at minimum interventions require addressing quality of life and follow-up over realistic time frames. Qualitative studies could helpfully inform more participant-focused assessment of treatment outcome with the addition of observed events such as improved overall health; enrolment in further education, training, or work; and participation in community or society.

In conclusion, all RCTs we found in this systematic review and meta-analysis were of psychological interventions. Small improvements for intervention over control were found for PTSD symptoms and functioning after treatment but not at follow-up, nor was any improvement evident for psychological distress at either time point or for quality of life at the end of treatment. The overall confidence in these results and precision of estimate is still less than satisfactory, and further studies are likely to change the estimates of effect, but the differences between our findings and the impression of treatment effectiveness from narrative reviews are substantial and suggest that more survivor-focused conceptualisation of problems and improved methodology are needed.

Supporting information

S1 PRISMA Checklist [doc]
PRISMA, Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses.

S1 Text [docx]
Systematic review protocol.

S1 Table [docx]
Characteristics of included studies.


Zdroje

1. United Nations General Assembly. Convention against torture and other cruel, inhuman or degrading treatment or punishment. Constitution. 1984 Dec;5(7):5.

2. Amnesty International. Torture in 2014: 30 years of broken promises. Amnesty International. 2014. http://www.amnesty.org/en/library/info/ACT40/004/2014/en. [cited 21 June 2019].

3. Allansson M, Melander E, Themnér L. Organized violence, 1989–2016. J Peace Res. 2017 Jul;54(4):574–87.

4. Patel N, Granville-Chapman C. Health Assessments with Survivors of Torture: Good Practice Clinical Guidelines. 2010. Joint publication of Medical Foundation, London and Department of Health, United Kingdom.

5. Song SJ, Subica A, Kaplan C, Tol W, de Jong J. Predicting the mental health and functioning of torture survivors. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2018 Jan 1;206(1):33–9. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000678 28350563

6. Jaranson JM, Quiroga J. Evaluating the services of torture rehabilitation programmes. Torture. 2011;21(2):98–140. 21715958

7. Williams AC, Amris K. Treatment of persistent pain from torture: review and commentary. Med Confl Surviv. 2017 Jan 2;33(1):60–81. doi: 10.1080/13623699.2016.1242050 27737567

8. Miller KE, Rasmussen A. War exposure, daily stressors, and mental health in conflict and post-conflict settings: bridging the divide between trauma-focused and psychosocial frameworks. Soc Sci Med. 2010 Jan 1;70(1):7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.09.029 19854552

9. Porter M, Haslam N. Predisplacement and postdisplacement factors associated with mental health of refugees and internally displaced persons: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2005 Aug 3;294(5):602–12. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.5.602 16077055

10. Esala JJ, Vukovich MM, Hanbury A, Kashyap S, Joscelyne A. Collaborative care for refugees and torture survivors: Key findings from the literature. Traumatology. 2018 Sep;24(3):168.

11. Shannon PJ, Wieling E, Simmelink-McCleary J, Becher E. Beyond stigma: Barriers to discussing mental health in refugee populations. J Loss Trauma. 2015 May 4;20(3):281–96.

12. Patel N, Kellezi B, de C Williams AC. Psychological, social and welfare interventions for psychological health and well‐being of torture survivors. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014;(11): Art. No.: CD009317.

13. Morina N, Malek M, Nickerson A, Bryant RA. Meta‐analysis of interventions for posttraumatic stress disorder and depression in adult survivors of mass violence in low‐and middle‐income countries. Depress Anxiety. 2017 Aug;34(8):679–91. doi: 10.1002/da.22618 28419625

14. Weiss WM, Ugueto AM, Mahmooth Z, Murray LK, Hall BJ, Nadison M, et al. Mental health interventions and priorities for research for adult survivors of torture and systematic violence: a review of the literature. Torture. 2016;26(1):17–44. 27857003

15. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009; 6(7):e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 19621072

16. Higgins JP. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Version 5.1. 0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration. www.cochrane-handbook.org 2011.

17. Bass J, Murray SM, Mohammed TA, Bunn M, Gorman W, Ahmed AM, et al. A randomized controlled trial of a trauma-informed support, skills, and psychoeducation intervention for survivors of torture and related trauma in Kurdistan, Northern Iraq. Glob Health Sci Pract. 2016 Sep 28;4(3):452–66. doi: 10.9745/GHSP-D-16-00017 27609624

18. Bolton P, Lee C, Haroz EE, Murray L, Dorsey S, Robinson C, et al. A transdiagnostic community-based mental health treatment for comorbid disorders: development and outcomes of a randomized controlled trial among Burmese refugees in Thailand. PLoS Med. 2014 Nov 11;11(11):e1001757. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001757 25386945

19. Esala JJ, Taing S. Testimony therapy with ritual: a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Trauma Stress. 2017 Feb;30(1):94–8. doi: 10.1002/jts.22163 28084662

20. Puvimanasinghe TS, Price IR. Healing through giving testimony: An empirical study with Sri Lankan torture survivors. Transcult Psychiatry. 2016 Oct;53(5):531–50. doi: 10.1177/1363461516651361 27330026

21. Wang SJ, Bytyçi A, Izeti S, Kallaba M, Rushiti F, Montgomery E, et al. A novel bio-psycho-social approach for rehabilitation of traumatized victims of torture and war in the post-conflict context: a pilot randomized controlled trial in Kosovo. Confl Health. 2016 Dec;10(1):34.

22. Weiss WM, Murray LK, Zangana GA, Mahmooth Z, Kaysen D, Dorsey S, et al. Community-based mental health treatments for survivors of torture and militant attacks in Southern Iraq: a randomized control trial. BMC Psychiatry. 2015 Dec;15(1):249.

23. Bichescu D, Neuner F, Schauer M, Elbert T. Narrative exposure therapy for political imprisonment-related chronic posttraumatic stress disorder and depression. Behav Res Ther. 2007 Sep 1;45(9):2212–20. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.12.006 17288990

24. Hensel-Dittmann D, Schauer M, Ruf M, Catani C, Odenwald M, Elbert T, et al. Treatment of traumatized victims of war and torture: a randomized controlled comparison of narrative exposure therapy and stress inoculation training. Psychother Psychosom. 2011;80(6):345–52. doi: 10.1159/000327253 21829046

25. Igreja V, Kleijn WC, Schreuder BJ, Van Dijk JA, Verschuur M. Testimony method to ameliorate post-traumatic stress symptoms: Community-based intervention study with Mozambican civil war survivors. Br J Psychiatry. 2004 Mar;184(3):251–7.

26. Neuner F, Kurreck S, Ruf M, Odenwald M, Elbert T, Schauer M. Can asylum-seekers with posttraumatic stress disorder be successfully treated? A randomized controlled pilot study. Cogn Behav Ther. 2010 Jan 1;39(2):81–91. doi: 10.1080/16506070903121042 19816834

27. Paunovic N, Öst LG. Cognitive-behavior therapy vs exposure therapy in the treatment of PTSD in refugees. Behav Res Ther. 2001 Oct 1;39(10):1183–97. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(00)00093-0 11579988

28. Pokhariyal GP, Rono R, Munywoki S. Analysis of treatment methods for victims of torture in Kenya and East Africa region. Traumatology. 2013 Jun;19(2):107–17.

29. Schauer M, Elbert T, Gotthardt S, Rockstroh B, Odenwald M, Neuner F. Imaginary reliving in psychotherapy modifies mind and brain. Verhaltenstherapie. 2006 Jan 1;16(2):96–103.

30. ter Heide FJ, Mooren T, Kleijn W, de Jongh A, Kleber R. EMDR versus stabilisation in traumatised asylum seekers and refugees: Results of a pilot study. Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2011 Jan 1;2(1):5881.

31. Yeomans PD, Forman EM, Herbert JD, Yuen E. A randomized trial of a reconciliation workshop with and without PTSD psychoeducation in Burundian sample. J Trauma Stress. 2010 Jun;23(3):305–12. doi: 10.1002/jts.20531 20564362

32. Mollica RF, Caspi-Yavin Y, Bollini P, Truong T, Tor S, Lavelle J. The Harvard Trauma Questionnaire: validating a cross-cultural instrument for measuring torture, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1992 Feb;180(2):111–116. 1737972

33. Priebe S, Gavrilovic JJ, Bremner S, Ajdukovic D, Franciskovic T, Galeazzi GM, et al. Psychological symptoms as long-term consequences of war experiences. Psychopathology. 2013;46(1):45–54. doi: 10.1159/000338640 22890384

34. Miller KE, Rasmussen A. The mental health of civilians displaced by armed conflict: an ecological model of refugee distress. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2017 Apr;26(2):129–38. doi: 10.1017/S2045796016000172 27040595

35. Raghavan S, Rasmussen A, Rosenfeld B, Keller AS. Correlates of symptom reduction in treatment-seeking survivors of torture. Psychol Trauma. 2013 Jul;5(4):377.

36. McFarlane CA, Kaplan I. Evidence-based psychological interventions for adult survivors of torture and trauma: a 30-year review. Transcult Psychiatry. 2012 Jul;49(3–4):539–67. doi: 10.1177/1363461512447608 23008355

37. Salo CD, Bray EM. Empirically tested interventions for torture survivors: A systematic review through an ecological lens. Transl Issues Psychol Sci. 2016 Dec;2(4):449.

38. Crumlish N, O'Rourke K. A systematic review of treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder among refugees and asylum-seekers. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2010 Apr 1;198(4):237–51. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181d61258 20386252

39. Nicholl C, Thompson A. The psychological treatment of Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) in adult refugees: A review of the current state of psychological therapies. J Ment Health. 2004 Aug 1;13(4):351–62.

40. Bronfenbrenner U. Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Am Psychol. 1977 Jul;32(7):513.

41. Sousa VD, Rojjanasrirat W. Translation, adaptation and validation of instruments or scales for use in cross‐cultural health care research: a clear and user‐friendly guideline. J Eval Clin Pract. 2011 Apr;17(2):268–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2010.01434.x 20874835

42. Pilkington K, Boshnakova A, Clarke M, Richardson J. "No language restrictions" in database searches: what does this really mean? J Altern Complement Med. 2005 Feb 1;11(1):205–7. doi: 10.1089/acm.2005.11.205 15750383

43. Kieling C, Herrman H, Patel V, Mari JD. Indexation of psychiatric journals from low-and middle-income countries: a survey and a case study. World Psychiatry. 2009 Feb;8(1):40. 19293959

44. Ioannidis JP, Patsopoulos NA, Evangelou E. Uncertainty in heterogeneity estimates in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2007 Nov 1;335(7626):914–6. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39343.408449.80 17974687

45. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta‐analysis. Stat Med. 2002 Jun 15;21(11):1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186 12111919

46. Inter-Agency Standing Committee. IASC guidelines on mental health and psychosocial support in emergency settings. Geneva, Switzerland: IASC 2006.

47. Esala JJ, Vukovich MM, Hanbury A, Kashyap S, Joscelyne A. Collaborative care for refugees and torture survivors: Key findings from the literature. Traumatology. 2018 Sep;24(3):168.

48. Goodkind JR. Promoting Hmong refugees’ well-being through mutual learning: Valuing knowledge, culture, and experience. Am J Community Psychol. 2006 Mar 1;37(1–2):77. doi: 10.1007/s10464-005-9003-6 16680538

49. Morley S. Single case methods in clinical psychology: A practical guide. Routledge; 2017 Dec 22.

Štítky
Interné lekárstvo

Článok vyšiel v časopise

PLOS Medicine


2019 Číslo 9
Najčítanejšie tento týždeň
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvýšte si kvalifikáciu online z pohodlia domova

Aktuální možnosti diagnostiky a léčby litiáz
nový kurz
Autori: MUDr. Tomáš Ürge, PhD.

Všetky kurzy
Prihlásenie
Zabudnuté heslo

Zadajte e-mailovú adresu, s ktorou ste vytvárali účet. Budú Vám na ňu zasielané informácie k nastaveniu nového hesla.

Prihlásenie

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte sa

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#