Assertive, trainable and older dogs are perceived as more dominant in multi-dog households
Authors:
Lisa J. Wallis aff001; Ivaylo B. Iotchev aff001; Enikő Kubinyi aff001
Authors place of work:
Department of Ethology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
aff001
Published in the journal:
PLoS ONE 15(1)
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227253
Summary
Social dominance is an important and widely used concept, however, different interpretations have led to ambiguity in the scientific literature and in popular science. Even though in ethology dominance is an attribute of dyadic encounters, and not a characteristic of the individual, ‘dominance’ has often been referred to as a personality trait in animals. Since few studies have specifically examined the link between personality traits and dominance status, we investigated this in dogs living in multi-dog households using a questionnaire, which required owners to specify whether the dog had a dominant or submissive status, and comprised items of both the features of the individual (i.e. personality traits) and previous social experience (interactions with group members and strangers). Four distinct personality factors emerged from 23 behavioural items by principal component analysis, labelled as assertiveness, trainability, intraspecific aggression and independence. Binomial logistic regression was used to examine how the demographic information of the dogs and the personality factors predicted the owner’s estimate of the dog’ status as dominant or submissive. The personality factor assertiveness accounted for 34% of the variance in dominance status, trainability 5% and dog age contributed 4%. Dogs perceived as dominant scored more highly on the factors assertiveness and trainability, which can help explain why ‘dominance’ has often been suggested to be a personality trait, rather than a dyad-specific social status according to different traditions in behavioural research. Similar to the ‘social dominance’ trait in humans, owner ascribed dominance showed a quadratic trajectory in cross-sectional mean change across the lifespan, increasing during adulthood and then maintaining high levels until old age. Overall, our study proposes a multifactorial background of dominance relationships in pet dogs, suggesting that not only previous experience of social interactions between individuals but also age and personality traits influence owner perceived dominance status in multi-dog households.
Keywords:
Pets and companion animals – Dogs – Animal behavior – Questionnaires – Aggression – Personality – Personality traits – Personality tests
Introduction
Dominance is used to describe social relationships among group-living animals. It is an important and widely used concept, however, there is little agreement regarding its meaning, and different interpretations have often led to ambiguity in the scientific literature and in popular science. In ethology, dominance is a relative measure, an attribute of dyadic encounters and refers to a consistent outcome in favour of the same dyad member and a default yielding response of its opponent rather than escalation [1]. Dominance status is typically determined by examining the outcomes of aggressive interactions within dyads (agonistic dominance), ritualized and/or greeting signals that are independent of context (formal dominance) and motivation to obtain valued resources (competitive ability, measured through pairwise competition tests). When dominance is operationalized as competitive ability, the consistent winner is dominant, and the loser—subordinate.
Historically, in human psychology, social dominance is considered as an aspect or trait of personality [2]. Personality describes a specific pattern of behaviour, thoughts, and feelings that persist through time and across situations [3]. Personality traits refer to measurable aspects of personality that vary between individuals, but remain relatively consistent within individuals across time and context [4]. Factor analysis (or principle component analysis) can be used to identify personality factors/traits that are robust across investigations, samples and time. Confusingly, in some studies in animals, if a behavioural factor identified through factor analysis associated with dominance status or rank, it was often labelled as ‘dominance’ [5], which contributed to the ambiguity around the term, leading to the assumption that dominance is a personality trait. For example, meta-analyses of research on temperament and personality (the two terms are often used interchangeably [6]) traits in dogs have suggested that across studies, ‘social dominance’ (a terminology adopted by e.g. [7–9]), is one of six [10], or seven recurring personality factors investigated in the dog [11]. Jones and Gosling [11] found that dominance was characterised by behaviours such as refusing to move out of the way, bullying other dogs, guarding food, and eating first. Therefore, dominance is often referred to as a personality trait of dogs, both in the literature [2] and by laymen [12,13]. This misuse of the term has contributed to the rise of dominance related aversive training techniques, such as hitting, shaking, growling, staring, and using other physical force, such as the ‘alpha roll’, and the ‘dominance down’; all of which in most cases provoke fearful or defensively aggressive behaviour in the dog [14].
Ethologists have argued against the existence of dominance as a personality trait. Individuals living in a group can be dominant or submissive with different partners, and thus dominance status within dyads is flexible, which does not fit to the definition of personality traits. In some relationships, the context of the behavioural interaction proves important. For example, individual competitive ability and differences in motivation to obtain valued resources can interact to produce different outcomes in different contexts. Relationships between dyads also involves affiliative behaviours in addition to, or even in the absence of dominance behaviours. Some dyads avoid each other and thus do not interact, so it is not possible to easily determine their relationship [15]. The fact that animals can form complex dynamic relationships that differ between dyads argues against the concept of dominance (and submissiveness) as a personality trait.
The relationship between dominance rank (position in a hierarchy) and related behaviours has been investigated in many species (for example; elephants (Loxodonta africana and Elephas maximus) [16], bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) [17], chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) [18–21], dogs (Canis familiaris) [15,22–27], hyena (Crocuta crocuta) [28], gorilla (Gorilla gorilla) [29–31], female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) [32], great tits (Parus major) [33], mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli) [34], starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) [35], barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis) [36], male rainbowfish (Melanotaenia duboulayi) [37], and brown trout (Salmo trutta) [38]). However, far fewer studies have attempted to determine whether natural variation in personality can predict social status [27,37,39]. This is particularly surprising given the theoretical links between evolutionary game theory and the maintenance of animal personality [40]. Such studies would help clarify the correct terminology and ensure that personality traits/factors and dominance status/rank are not treated equivalently.
One suitable species to examine the link between personality traits and dominance status is the domestic dog. Many households contain more than one dog, which allows owners to observe the formation of dominance relationships. Even in single dog households, the emergence of dog parks has facilitated socialisation between dogs, which has enabled owners to view interactions between familiar dogs on a regular basis. Several studies have already utilised owner questionnaires to determine the dominance status of dogs in multi-dog households [26,27,41,42]. When dominance is considered as an attribute of dyadic encounters, and not a property of individuals, the perception of each dogs’ status can be based on consistent patterns in the outcome of interactions within dyads [23,43]. For example, in multi-dog households, dogs perceived as dominant by the owner have priority access to certain resources (for example, resting places and food rewards), undertake specific tasks (defend the group during perceived threats and lead other dog/s during walks), display dominance (win fights and over mark), have characteristic personality traits (measured using single item statements), and are usually older than subordinates [26]. The fact that owner estimates of dominance status correspond to previously established behavioural markers of dominance displays in dogs, suggests that dominance relationships are robust and well-perceivable components of companion dog behaviour. It furthermore shows that owner-derived reports about dominance status have external validity. Additionally, there is evidence that owners scoring of their dogs’ behavioural traits via questionnaire is also valid, as it corresponds to observational measurements of behaviour. For example, extraverted dogs spent more time in dyads in off-leash parks, highly amicable dogs spent more time in play, and neurotic dogs displayed a higher frequency of lowered or hunched postures [44].
In free ranging and/or pet dogs, dominance status has been found to be associated with the personality factors motivation, trainability, sociability, impulsivity, and aggression. Importantly, the relationships tend to be less strong in larger dog packs and to be more specifically related to the formal dominance style [26,27,45]. Age, sex, leadership and reproductive success have also been found to be related to dominance status in free ranging and/or pet dogs [24,26,46–49]. Theoretical models predict that intra-specific dominance, especially when tied to consistent leadership, is only useful in small groups characterized by asymmetric distribution of experience and familiarity with the environment [50,51]. The more knowledgeable individual can convey a true advantage to other group members. Dominance follows almost automatically from this asymmetrical arrangement [23,52]. The likelihood of formal dominance and/or leadership can be expected to increase with age for this reason, since aged animals have more experience if the environment is stable across generations.
In the current study, we investigated how the owner perceived dominance rank of dogs living in multi-dog households is related to their personality traits, derived from a behavioural questionnaire by factor analysis, and dog demographic information using a pilot sample. Instead of using existing questionnaires, we developed a new one to include both the owners’ estimation of their dogs’ personality traits, and their dogs’ previous experience with other dogs in the household, and during meetings with other individuals. The questionnaire items were pre-selected to address behaviours frequently studied in dog personality research that might be related to dominance status [53]. We chose the terminology that owners most often use (e.g. ‘smart’ instead of ‘trainable’). In contrast to previous studies we used age in years to investigation nonlinear relationship with dominance rank. We hypothesized that dominant dogs as perceived by the owners will be older, and possess specific personality traits, such as high assertiveness, confidence or boldness, high physical aggression, and high trainability, in contrast to subordinate dogs.
Methods
Subjects
The questionnaire was filled in online by 396 owners of more than one dog (90.1% of which were women), for a total of 550 dogs, in Hungarian. The questionnaire was advertised in a social media Dog Ethology group, between 14th June 2014 and 6th February 2015, and specifically targeted owners with more than one dog in their household. Since we were interested in the personal knowledge and experience of the owner when examining owners’ perception of the dominance status of their dogs, we provided no training, explanation, or definition of dominance.
Dogs were on average 5.0 years old (± 3.13 SD), weighted 22.7 kg (± 13.54 SD), 54% of the total sample were female, and 54% of the total sample were neutered. Regarding breed, 4.4% were of unknown/mixed breed, and the most frequently present breeds in the sample were the German shepherd dog (8%), Border collie (6.0%), Vizsla (5.3%), Spaniel (including Cocker and Springer 4.7%), Dachshund (4.6%), Golden retriever (4.0%), Labrador retriever (3.6%), and Belgian shepherd (3.3%). All other breeds were represented by less than 3% of the sample. Please refer to the supplementary materials for tables with a breakdown of the breeds and breed groups (S1 and S2 Tables). According to dogs housing conditions, 26.6% of the dogs were kept in the house, 16.4% in the garden, and 57.0% both in the garden and in the house. We allocated the dogs to three groups according to their training level; 43.3% did not received any formal training, 28.1% received basic and 28.6% participated in specialised training (e.g. agility, hunting).
Procedure
The questionnaire consisted of demographic and keeping conditions questions about individual dogs (age in years, weight in kg, sex, neuter status, where the dog is kept, and training level), followed by one question about the perceived dominance status of the dog in the group (dominant N = 252, submissive, N = 116, both (or “I do not know”) N = 149, NA (missing data) N = 33 (the owner provided no answer to this question). The third part consisted of 23 questions about the dogs’ behaviour and personality (Table 1). For the questions about dog behaviour and personality we used a 1–5 scale system (Likert scale: 1 = no/not/never, 5 = very/very much/frequently).
Statistical analysis
Analyses were performed in SPSS 22.0 (factor analysis) and R (binomial logistic regression and graphs) [54]. In order to reduce the number of items, and to determine the underlying structure of the data, a principle component analysis (PCA) with Varimax rotation [55] and a default of maximum 25 iterations was used with the Maximum Likelihood method on the 23 questions concerning dog behaviour and personality traits on the full sample (N = 500, NA = 50). For details please refer to supplementary materials S1 File. The solution that explained >50% of the variance and with factor eigenvalues > 1 was accepted. In addition, items that did not load on any of the factors (below 0.5) were removed from the analysis. Subsequently, the items of each final factor were tested for internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha. The resulting factor structure was then used as a template to calculate the factor scores for each individual dog, in order to allow missing values, with the provision of a minimum of two values per factor, to maximise the sample size (N = 542). We calculated the trait scores by taking the mean of the items loading with at least 0.5 on a given factor (items that loaded negatively on a factor were inverted (e.g. calm and socialized)).
We then used binomial logistic regression to test how the demographic information of the dogs (age in years, weight in kg, sex, neuter status, where the dog is kept, and training level), and the factors obtained with the personality trait factor analysis would predict the owner’s estimate of the dog as dominant or submissive (rank status). After removing dogs that were identified as “both” (sometimes dominant, sometimes submissive (N = 182)) and those with missing information ((NA) N = 28), the sample size using the full model was 332 individuals, and in the reduced model 343 individuals were included. Due to the small sample size, we only examined main effects, but included quadratic terms for the continuous personality factor scores and age in years. Non-significant terms (P > 0.05) were removed stepwise from the model. A pseudo R-squared value was calculated to determine how well the model explains the data. The R package rcompanion and the command nagelkerke were used to produce a pseudo R squared value for the fixed effects in comparison to the intercept only model. For details of the R code, and results from the models please refer to supplementary materials S2 File. Possible dependence between owner responses of dogs living in the same household was addressed in the supplementary materials (S3 File). Graphs were produced in R using the ggplot package and the geom_smooth function to plot the smoothed conditional mean and confidence intervals. Significant predictors were mean centred before plotting, by subtracting the sample mean from each observation, in order to make the intercept more meaningful.
Ethics statement
The data was collected using an online questionnaire designed to assess the dogs’ demographic data, personality, and keeping conditions via owner report. According to the current Hungarian law (1998. évi XXVIII. Törvény—the Animal Protection Act, 3rd paragraph, 9th point), non-invasive observational data collection on dog demographics and behaviour are not considered as animal experiments and are therefore allowed to be conducted without any special permission from the University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (UIACUC). The filling out of the questionnaires was voluntary and anonymous so the study did not violate respondents' privacy. Informed consent was included in the introductory text of the questionnaires. Ethical approval or an ethical waiver from an Institutional Research Board or equivalent for collecting survey data from human participants was not necessary, as we did not collect private, identifiable information about human third parties.
Results
Factor analysis of dog characteristics, behaviours and previous social experience questionnaire items
Seventeen items contributing to four factors were found to explain 57.8% of the total variance (Table 2), while six items were excluded (listed in short form: fit, best rest, slow, mount others, adaptive, and novelty seeking). The factors were labelled as assertiveness (22.77% of variance explained, Cronbach alpha = 0.76), trainability (16.88% of variance explained, Cronbach alpha = 0.73), intraspecific aggression (10.48% of variance explained, Cronbach alpha = 0.73), and independence (7.71% of variance explained, Cronbach alpha = 0.73). The loadings of the items on the factors are shown in Table 2.
Descriptive information of the canine personality factors
The intraspecific aggression factor was positively skewed, with half the dogs scoring between 1.80 and 3.33. Trainability was the most negatively skewed of the factors, with half the dogs scoring between 3.75 to 4.75. At least one dog obtained the maximum score possible on each of the four factors, apart from for intraspecific aggression. The largest range of scores was obtained for the assertiveness and independence factors while the intraspecific aggression factor had the smallest range. The median scores and percentiles for each of the personality trait factors are shown in Fig 1.
Binomial logistic regression of dominance status, dog demographics and PCA factors
The final model revealed significant associations with the personality trait factor scales of assertiveness and trainability, as well as dog age in years. We calculated the pseudo R-squared measure to indicate how well the model explains the data. McFadden pseudo R squared was 0.43 indicating an excellent model fit.
A significant polynomial relationship between the factor score assertiveness and the probability that dogs were allocated a ‘dominant’ or ‘subordinate’ status by the owner was found. Dogs that were described as more assertive were significantly more likely to be dominant, than dogs that scored lower in assertiveness (see Fig 2, Table 3). Dogs that had a higher than average assertiveness score (3.29) had a greater than 75% probability of being allocated a “dominant” status. On average, a one-unit change in assertiveness is associated with an exp(41.248−2×-10.479) change in the odds of being dominant.
Additionally, we found a significant linear relationship between the factor scale trainability and the proportion of dogs that were allocated a ‘dominant’ status. Dogs that were described as more trainable were significantly more likely to be labelled dominant, than dogs that scored lower in trainability (see Fig 3, Table 3). Holding all other predictors at a fixed value, we see a 73% increase in the odds of being dominant for a one-unit increase in trainability score.
Finally, we found a significant quadratic relationship between the demographic variable age in years and the probability that dogs were allocated a dominant status. Dogs that were older were significantly more likely to be dominant according to the owner, than dogs that were younger (see Fig 4, Table 3). Dogs of one year of age had around a 50% probability of being dominant, which rose to 80% at the age of eight. By age 10, the probability of being dominant began to plateau, and the youngest and oldest dogs showed the greatest variability. On average, a one-year change in age is associated with an exp(12.408−2×-8.167) change in the odds of being dominant.
Weight, sex, neuter status, training level, keeping place, and the personality trait factors intraspecific aggression and independence had no significant effect on the estimated dominance rank.
Discussion
The current study aimed to identify demographic and personality factors associated with dogs’ dominance status as perceived by the owner using a questionnaire. The main finding is that dogs assigned a dominant status displayed higher assertiveness and trainability and were older than subordinate dogs. The remaining two traits, intraspecific aggression and independence, weight, sex, neuter status, where the dog is kept, and training level had no association with owner perceived dominance status.
The factors trainability and (intraspecific) aggression correspond to already established personality factors in dogs (reviews: 8 studies aggression, 11 responsiveness to training [51], and 30 aggression and 34 responsiveness to training [52]). However, independence and assertiveness are less widespread. Independence refers to the dogs’ tendency to make decisions independently of the owner. Two previous studies have identified a factor/items which they also labelled or described as “independence” [56,57]. Assertiveness’ associated items point to perceived confidence, initiative and persistence in social interactions. It has been described in other species, but so far has only been suggested to be linked to dominance status in dogs [58]. However, previous studies have defined analogous traits to assertiveness, such as ‘boldness’ [59–61] (which increases with age in dogs [62,63]), and ‘confidence’, ‘courage’, ‘self-confidence’, and ‘motivation’ [64–68].
The fact that dogs perceived as dominant were more assertive is not surprising. Previous studies have shown that dominant individuals undertake specific tasks such as defending the group during perceived threats and leading other dogs during walks (which corresponds to the items ‘pack defence’ and ‘leading type’). They also display dominance through consistently winning fights (item ‘wins play fights’) [26]. Interestingly, in free ranging dogs, participation in intergroup conflicts increases with a decreasing ratio of the number of rivals [69], and the number of affiliative partners involved [70]. Given that most dogs living in multi-dog households have relatively strong affiliative bonds, and most interactions with strangers occur in small groups, or singularly, this might help explain why some owners observed higher levels of pack defence and leading in the more dominant animals.
In their review, Gosling and John [5] found that the personality factor ‘dominance’ emerged as a clear factor in 7 animal studies out of 19. Although this factor was interpreted as ‘confidence’ in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and ‘assertiveness’ in hyenas (Crocuta crocuta), all measures correlated substantially with dominance rank. This suggests that these personality factors (labelled dominance, assertiveness, confidence etc. in previous studies) describe typical dominance related behaviours that are displayed to familiar individuals that live in a group setting. For example, assertiveness also corresponds to the previously described trait of ‘motivation’ in dogs [67,68], as suggested by Bradshaw et al. [71].
Highly trainable dogs also tended to be allocated a dominant status by owners. Dogs high in trainability were reported to be smart, fast learners, could “read people” well, and were obedient. Previously we found that dogs allocated a dominant status within dyads were rated as “smarter” than subordinates [26] and more controllable [27]. In addition, studies have demonstrated rank-related effects on cognitive ability. For example, in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) and pheasants (Phasianus colchicus), the fastest learners occupied the highest competitive ranks [35,72], and in rhesus macaques, dominant individuals showed superior learning capacities when tested, and subordinates “played dumb” when learning in mixed social groups. The subordinates avoided socially difficult situations by inhibiting their behaviour, and missed out on desirable food items, in order to minimise potential retaliation from dominant individuals [73]. Therefore, it is conceivable that in the current study, subordinate dogs are not less smart but inhibit their behaviour in the home environment in order to avoid conflicts with the dominant animal.
In contrast to our predictions, dogs labelled as dominant by owners did not show higher scores in the factor ‘intraspecific aggression’. Some dogs allocated a dominant status by the owner may have a formal dominance relationship (display ritualized and/or greeting signals that are independent of context) with the other dog in the household and adopt a non-confrontational attitude with stranger dogs, and thus may rarely show aggression, or they may have a non-interactive relationship with other dogs, which would also result in low intraspecific aggression scores. Indeed, field studies in both dogs and macaques [49,74] suggest that agonistic dominance (associated with higher aggression) is less likely than formal dominance to predict leadership. Aggressive interactions are usually influenced by motivation and context (e.g. reproductive activity) [75], and occur more often in less well-established relationships, and therefore may not correspond to the underlying hierarchy [24,76]. Hence, our results imply that individuals rated as dominant by owners were more likely expressing formal dominance.
Although part of this study’s aim was to additionally assess the influence of demographic and keeping conditions on dominance, none of the chosen factors (weight in kg, sex, neuter status, where the dog is kept, and training level) reached significance. The only exception was the age of the dog in years. Overall, we found a quadratic relationship between age in years and the probability that dogs were allocated a dominant status. In humans, the personality trait ‘social dominance’ shows a very similar quadratic trajectory in cross-sectional mean trait change across adulthood [77], as does ‘dominance’ in male chimpanzees [78]. Age also predicted dominance rank in our previous study [26], where overall 66% of dominant individuals were the older animal in the dyad.
Older dogs were more likely to be classified as dominant than younger dogs, in agreement with the literature for both wolves and dogs [22,23,46,47,79]. One-year old dogs had a 50% probability of being classified as dominant, which seems high given their age and amount of experience. However, since we do not know the age of their partner dog/s, perhaps they were the oldest in a group of young dogs, or they were all a similar age. Interestingly, in free-ranging dogs, subadults (who tend to be in the middle of the hierarchy) target more dominance interactions of all types at other subadult individuals, which may indicate instability in the hierarchy [76]. Although adolescent dogs may be sexually mature, dogs do not tend to show fully adult behaviour until 2–3 years of age [80]. Most adolescent dogs go through a hormonal surge which affects their behaviour, decreasing their ability to pay attention and respond to previously learned cues [81,82]. High activity levels and motivation, as well as deficiencies in executive control during this period, might result in owners interpreting their dog’s behaviour as an expression of dominance, and lead them to conclude that the dog is dominant.
An important limitation of the current study is that 90% of the owners surveyed were Hungarian females, which prevented the examination of sex effects. Previous studies have found owner sex differences in the perception of dog behaviour; male owners perceive their dogs to be more disobedient [83], bold, and less sociable and trainable in comparison to female owners [63]. Therefore, there are likely to be differences in the way male and female owners perceive dominance rank and related behaviours. Future studies should aim to include more males, people from other countries/cultures, and in addition, they should take into account the characteristics of the other canine members of the group (including sex, breed and age).
This study focused on examining owner perceived dominance status, associated dominance related behaviours, and personality factors, and did not attempt to define all the different types of relationships found previously in dogs. For example, we were not able to determine whether dogs’ relationships were characterised by agonistic dominance, formal dominance, or competitive ability, which might differ between contexts. Although the observed pattern of low intra-specific aggression, high trainability and higher age of the dominant animals are found in the literature on free-ranging dogs and are particularly associated with the formal type, additional studies are necessary to clarify the relationship between dominance status, dominance related behaviours, and associated personality factors in dogs. Furthermore, how owner demographics (e.g. the number of people in the household, their amount of experience with dogs, their sex, age and/or personalities) influence how they perceive dominance and dominance related behaviours between dogs in their household, should also be established. To obtain a more complete picture of the factors influencing social relationships in dogs, questions pertaining to agonistic and formal dominance behaviours, previous experiences, social contexts, play, affiliation, passive interactions, sleeping proximities, and other tactile communication within multi-dog households should be addressed [84].
Conclusion
Overall, our study suggests a multifactorial background of dominance relationships in pet dogs. It is likely that dominance status is not only determined by previous experience of social interactions between group members as suggested by ethologists but also by personality factors as proposed by psychologists [85]. Since owners based their answers on their estimation of their dogs’ characteristics, and previous experience with their dog’s behaviour when socialising with other dogs in the household, and during meetings with other individuals, our study combined both the features of the individual and previous intraspecific social interactions. Our work adds to a growing line of evidence that some personality factors, namely assertiveness and trainability can increase the odds that an owner ascribes the dog a higher status within the household. The identified link between assertiveness as a personality trait and dominance status is probably the basis of the confusion regarding the term ‘dominance’ among lay people and also partially in science. Furthermore, while future studies need to more directly address the distinction between different dominance styles, the observation that assertiveness was a better predictor of dominance status than intraspecific aggression in our study, suggests that mainly formal dominance (i.e. dominance displayed without aggression) influences owners’ perception of dogs’ social rank in multi-dog households. Finally, the weight, sex, neuter status, and training level of the dog did not influence owner perceived dominance status.
Importantly, our results suggest that labelling an animal personality factor as ‘dominance’ is an incorrect use of the term. Therefore, in order to facilitate the discrimination between personality traits and dominance as a status in dyads, or as a rank in dog social groups, we suggest using ‘assertive’ when describing related personality traits, and ‘dominance’ when referring to status/ranks between individuals with an established relationship.
Supporting information
S1 Table [docx]
Break down of all the dog breeds present in the sample, including count of the dogs and the percentage of the overall sample.
S2 Table [docx]
Count of the number of dogs in each breed group of the Fédération Cynologique Internationale (FCI), including the percentage of the overall sample.
S1 File [pca]
Principle component analysis.
S2 File [docx]
R code.
S3 File [docx]
GEE models.
Zdroje
1. Drews C. The Concept and Definition of Dominance in Animal Behaviour. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, editor. Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1993;125: 283–313. doi: 10.1163/156853993X00290
2. Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006;132: 1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1 16435954
3. John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. Handb Personal Theory Res. 2008;3: 881.
4. Bell AM, Hankison SJ, Laskowski KL. The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2009;77: 771–783. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.022 24707058
5. Gosling SD, John OP. Personality Dimensions in Nonhuman Animals. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1999;8: 69–75. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00017
6. McCrae RR, Costa PT, Ostendorf F, Angleitner a, Hrebícková M, Avia MD, et al. Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78: 173–86. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10653513
7. James WT. Social organization among dogs of different temperaments, terriers and beagles, reared together. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1951;44: 71–77. doi: 10.1037/h0061218 14814243
8. Beaudet R, Chalifoux A, Dallaire A. Predictive value of activity level and behavioral evaluation on future dominance in puppies. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1994;40: 273–284. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(94)90068-X
9. Campbell WE. A behaviour test for puppy selection. Mod Vet Pract. [American Veterinary Publications]; 1972;53: 29–33. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5080472
10. Fratkin JL, Sinn DL, Patall EA, Gosling SD. Personality Consistency in Dogs: A Meta-Analysis. Widdig A, editor. PLoS One. 2013;8: e54907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054907 23372787
11. Jones AC, Gosling SD. Temperament and personality in dogs (Canis familiaris): A review and evaluation of past research. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2005;95: 1–53. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.04.008
12. Dan D. Dominant Vs Submissive Dog Personality Traits [Internet]. 2018. https://theonlinedogtrainer.com/best-behaved-dog-breeds-does-breed-determine-a-dogs-personality/
13. Familypet. What is the definition of a dominant dog [Internet]. https://familypet.com/what-is-the-definition-of-a-dominant-dog/
14. Herron ME, Shofer FS, Reisner IR. Survey of the use and outcome of confrontational and non-confrontational training methods in client-owned dogs showing undesired behaviors. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;117: 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.12.011
15. Smuts B, Sandel AA, Trisko RK. Affiliation, dominance and friendship among companion dogs. Behaviour. 2016;153: 693–725. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003352
16. Yasui S, Konno A, Tanaka M, Idani G, Ludwig A, Lieckfeldt D, et al. Personality Assessment and Its Association With Genetic Factors in Captive Asian and African Elephants. 2013; doi: 10.1002/zoo.21045 22996044
17. Frick EE, Frick EE. Establishing a Link Between Personality and Social Rank in a Group of Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) by. 2016;
18. Anestis SF. Behavioral style, dominance rank, and urinary cortisol in young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). 2005; 1245–1268.
19. Buirski P, Plutchik R, Kellerman H. Sex differences, dominance, and personality in the chimpanzee. Anim Behav. 1978;26: 123–129. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90011-8 565175
20. Pederson AK, King JE, Landau VI. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality predicts behavior. J Res Pers. 2005;39: 534–549. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.07.002
21. King JE, Figueredo AJ. The Five-Factor Model plus Dominance in Chimpanzee Personality. J Res Pers. 1997;31: 257–271. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1997.2179
22. Trisko RK, Smuts BB. Dominance relationships in a group of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Behaviour. 2015;152: 677–704. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003249
23. Bonanni R, Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. Effect of affiliative and agonistic relationships on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2010;79: 981–991. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.021
24. Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P, Bonanni R, Natoli E. Dominance in relation to age, sex, and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behav Ecol. 2010;21: 443–455. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq001
25. Bonanni R, Cafazzo S. The Social Organisation of a Population of Free-Ranging Dogs in a Suburban Area of Rome. The Social Dog. Elsevier; 2014. pp. 65–104.
26. Kubinyi E, Wallis LJ. Dominance in dogs as rated by owners corresponds to ethologically valid markers of dominance. PeerJ. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory; 2019;7: e6838. doi: 10.7717/peerj.6838 31119074
27. Ákos Z, Beck R, Nagy M, Vicsek T, Kubinyi E. Leadership and Path Characteristics during Walks Are Linked to Dominance Order and Individual Traits in Dogs. Faisal AA, editor. PLoS Comput Biol. 2014;10: e1003446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003446 24465200
28. Gosling SD. Personality Dimensions in Spotted Hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). J Comp Psychol. 1998;112: 107–118. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.107 9642781
29. Gold KC, Maple TL. Personality Assessment in the Gorilla and Its Utility As a Management Tool. 1994;522.
30. Eckardt W, Steklis HD, Steklis NG, Fletcher AW, Stoinski TS, Weiss A. Personality dimensions and their behavioral correlates in wild Virunga mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). J Comp Psychol. 2015;129: 26–41. doi: 10.1037/a0038370 25528652
31. Kuhar CW, Stoinski TS, Lukas KE, Maple TL. Gorilla Behavior Index revisited: Age, housing and behavior. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2006;96: 315–326. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.06.004
32. David M, Auclair Y, Cézilly F. Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding context. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2011;81: 219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.10.008
33. Dingemanse NJ, De Goede P. The relation between dominance and exploratory behavior is context-dependent in wild great tits. Behav Ecol. 2004;15: 1023–1030. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arh115
34. Fox RA, Ladage LD, Roth TC, Pravosudov VV. Behavioural profile predicts dominance status in mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli. Anim Behav. 2009;77: 1441–1448. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.02.022 20161203
35. Boogert NJ, Reader SM, Laland KN. The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings. Anim Behav. 2006;72: 1229–1239. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.02.021
36. Kurvers RHJM, Eijkelenkamp B, van Oers K, van Lith B, van Wieren SE, Ydenberg RC, et al. Personality differences explain leadership in barnacle geese. Anim Behav. 2009;78: 447–453. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.002
37. Colléter M, Brown C. Personality traits predict hierarchy rank in male rainbowfish social groups. Anim Behav. 2011;81: 1231–1237. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.03.011
38. Adriaenssens B, Johnsson JI. Shy trout grow faster: Exploring links between personality and fitness-related traits in the wild. Behav Ecol. 2011;22: 135–143. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq185
39. David M, Auclair Y, Cézilly F. Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding context. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2011;81: 219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.10.008
40. Briffa M, Sneddon LU, Wilson AJ. Animal personality as a cause and consequence of contest behaviour. Biol Lett. 2015;11. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.1007 25808004
41. Pongrácz P, Bánhegyi P, Miklósi Á. When rank counts—dominant dogs learn better from a human demonstrator in a two-action test. Behaviour. 2012;149: 111–132. doi: 10.1163/156853912X629148
42. Pongrácz P, Vida V, Bánhegyi P, Miklósi A. How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)? Anim Cogn. 2008;11: 75–82. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0090-7 17492317
43. Schenkel R. Submission: Its features and function in the wolf and dog. Integr Comp Biol. 1967;7: 319–329. doi: 10.1093/icb/7.2.319
44. Ottenheimer Carrier L, Cyr A, Anderson RE, Walsh CJ. Exploring the dog park: Relationships between social behaviours, personality and cortisol in companion dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. Elsevier B.V.; 2013;146: 96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.04.002
45. Muner Kroll Dantas de Castro M. Assessing the social organization of multi-dog households : Dog behaviour, hormones, personality, and demographics. Memorial University of Newfoundland. 2017.
46. Cafazzo S, Lazzaroni M, Marshall-Pescini S. Dominance relationships in a family pack of captive arctic wolves (Canis lupus arctos): the influence of competition for food, age and sex. PeerJ. 2016;4: e2707. doi: 10.7717/peerj.2707 27904806
47. Bonanni R, Cafazzo S, Abis A, Barillari E, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. Age-graded dominance hierarchies and social tolerance in packs of free-ranging dogs. Behav Ecol. 2017;28: 1004–1020. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arx059
48. Cafazzo S, Bonanni R, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. Social variables affecting mate preferences, copulation and reproductive outcome in a pack of free-ranging dogs. PLoS One. 2014;9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098594 24905360
49. Bonanni R, Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. Effect of affiliative and agonistic relationships on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2010;79: 981–991. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.021
50. Conradt L, Roper TJ. Consensus decision making in animals. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005;20: 449–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.008 16701416
51. Conradt L, Roper TJ. Group decision-making in animals. Nature. 2003;421: 155–158. doi: 10.1038/nature01294 12520299
52. de Waal FBM. Dominance “style” and primate social organization. Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and other Mammals. 1989. pp. 243–263.
53. Jones AC. Development and validation of a dog personality questionnaire [Internet]. The University of Texas at Austin, ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. University of Texas, Austin. 2008. http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=psyc6&NEWS=N&AN=2009-99140-320
54. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. http://www.r-project.org
55. Gudjonsson G. An easy guide to factor analysis. Pers Individ Dif. 1994;17: 302. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90040-X
56. Cattell RB, Korth B. The isolation of temperament dimensions in dogs. Behav Biol. 1973;9: 15–30. doi: 10.1016/s0091-6773(73)80165-8 4738708
57. Wahlgren K, Lester D. The big four: personality in dogs. Psychol Rep. Ammons Scientific; 2003;92: 828. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2003.92.3.828 12841450
58. Ley JM, Bennett PC. Understanding Personality by Understanding Companion Dogs. Anthrozoos A Multidiscip J Interact People Anim. 2007;20: 113–124. doi: 10.2752/175303707X207909
59. Gosling SD, Bonnenburg A V. An integrative approach to personality research in anthrozoology: Ratings of six species of pets and their owners. Anthrozoos. 1998;11: 148–155. doi: 10.2752/089279398787000661
60. Svartberg K, Forkman B. Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79: 133–155. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00121-1
61. Svartberg K. Shyness–boldness predicts performance in working dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79: 157–174.
62. Starling MJ, Branson N, Thomson PC, McGreevy PD. Age, sex and reproductive status affect boldness in dogs. Vet J. Elsevier Ltd; 2013;197: 868–872. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2013.05.019 23778256
63. Kubinyi E, Turcsán B, Miklósi A. Dog and owner demographic characteristics and dog personality trait associations. Behav Processes. 2009;81: 392–401. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.04.004 19520239
64. Humphrey ES. “Mental tests” for shepherd dogs: An attempted classification and evaluation of the various traits that go to make up “temperament” in the german shepherd dog. J Hered. Oxford University Press; 1934;25: 129–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a103899
65. Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. Individual variation in agonistic behaviour in dogs. Anim Behav. 1985;33: 1338–1342. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80195-0
66. Ruefenacht S, Gebhardt-Henrich S, Miyake T, Gaillard C. A behaviour test on German Shepherd dogs: heritability of seven different traits. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79: 113–132.
67. Ley JM, Bennett PC, Coleman GJ. A refinement and validation of the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;116: 220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.09.009
68. Ley J, Bennett P, Coleman G. Personality dimensions that emerge in companion canines. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;110: 305–317. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.04.016
69. Bonanni R, Natoli E, Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P. Free-ranging dogs assess the quantity of opponents in intergroup conflicts. Anim Cogn. 2011;14: 103–115. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0348-3 20845053
70. Bonanni R, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. Pattern of individual participation and cheating in conflicts between groups of free-ranging dogs. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2010;79: 957–968. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.016
71. Bradshaw JWS, Blackwell E, Casey RA. Dominance in domestic dogs—A response to Schilder et al. (2014). J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. Elsevier Inc; 2016;11: 102–108. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.11.008
72. Langley EJG, van Horik JO, Whiteside MA, Madden JR. Group social rank is associated with performance on a spatial learning task. R Soc Open Sci. 2018;5: 1–9. doi: 10.1098/rsos.171475 29515866
73. Drea CM, Wallen K. Low-status monkeys “play dumb” when learning in mixed social groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96: 12965–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.22.12965 10536031
74. De Waal FB. Macaque social culture: development and perpetuation of affiliative networks. J Comp Psychol. 1996;110: 147–154. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.110.2.147 8681528
75. Sands J, Creel S, State M, Sands J, Creel S, State M, et al. Social dominance, aggression and faecal glucocorticoid levels in a wild population of wolves, Canis lupus. Anim Behav. 2004;67: 387–396. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.03.019
76. Silk MJ, Cant MA, Cafazzo S, Natoli E, McDonald RA. Elevated aggression is associated with uncertainty in a network of dog dominance interactions. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2019;286: 20190536. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2019.0536 31266423
77. Roberts BW, Mroczek D. Personality Trait Change in Adulthood. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008;17: 31–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x 19756219
78. King JE, Weiss A, Sisco MM. Aping Humans: Age and Sex Effects in Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and Human (Homo sapiens) Personality. J Comp Psychol. 2008;122: 418–427. doi: 10.1037/a0013125 19014265
79. Mech LD. Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs. Can J Zool. 1999;77: 1196–1203. doi: 10.1139/z99-099
80. Miklósi Á. Dog behaviour, evolution, and cognition [Internet]. Oxford University Press.; 2008. https://books.google.com/books?id=h9rsmyaywPUC&pgis=1
81. Lindsay S. Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training, Volume Two [Internet]. Lindsay SR, editor. Assessment. Ames, Iowa, USA: Iowa State University Press; 2001.
82. Wallis LJ, Range F, Müller CA, Serisier S, Huber L, Zsófi V. Lifespan development of attentiveness in domestic dogs: drawing parallels with humans. Front Psychol. 2014;5: 71. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00071 24570668
83. Bennett PC, Rohlf VI. Owner-companion dog interactions: Relationships between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training engagement and shared activities. Appl Anim Behav Sci. Elsevier B.V.; 2007;102: 65–84. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.03.009
84. Overall KL. Special issue: The “dominance” debate and improved behavioral measures—Articles from the 2014 CSF/FSF. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. Elsevier Ltd; 2016;11: 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.12.004
85. Chase ID, Tovey C, Spangler-Martin D, Manfredonia M. Individual differences versus social dynamics in the formation of animal dominance hierarchies. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2002;99: 5744–5749. doi: 10.1073/pnas.082104199 11960030
Článok vyšiel v časopise
PLOS One
2020 Číslo 1
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
- Nejasný stín na plicích – kazuistika
- Masturbační chování žen v ČR − dotazníková studie
- Úspěšná resuscitativní thorakotomie v přednemocniční neodkladné péči
- Fixní kombinace paracetamol/kodein nabízí synergické analgetické účinky
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
- Psychometric validation of Czech version of the Sport Motivation Scale
- Comparison of Monocyte Distribution Width (MDW) and Procalcitonin for early recognition of sepsis
- Effects of supplemental creatine and guanidinoacetic acid on spatial memory and the brain of weaned Yucatan miniature pigs
- Accelerated sparsity based reconstruction of compressively sensed multichannel EEG signals