#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Quantification of speech and synchrony in the conversation of adults with autism spectrum disorder


Authors: Keiko Ochi aff001;  Nobutaka Ono aff002;  Keiho Owada aff003;  Masaki Kojima aff003;  Miho Kuroda aff003;  Shigeki Sagayama aff004;  Hidenori Yamasue aff005
Authors place of work: School of Media Science, Tokyo University of Technology, Hachioji, Japan aff001;  Department of Computer Science, Graduate School of Systems Design, Tokyo Metropolitan University, Hino, Japan aff002;  Department of Child Psychiatry, School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan aff003;  University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan aff004;  Department of Psychiatry, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Hamamatsu, Japan aff005
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(12)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225377

Summary

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a highly prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by impairments in social reciprocity and communication together with restricted interest and stereotyped behaviors. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) is considered a ‘gold standard’ instrument for diagnosis of ASD and mainly depends on subjective assessments made by trained clinicians. To develop a quantitative and objective surrogate marker for ASD symptoms, we investigated speech features including F0, speech rate, speaking time, and turn-taking gaps, extracted from footage recorded during a semi-structured socially interactive situation from ADOS. We calculated not only the statistic values in a whole session of the ADOS activity but also conducted a block analysis, computing the statistical values of the prosodic features in each 8s sliding window. The block analysis identified whether participants changed volume or pitch according to the flow of the conversation. We also measured the synchrony between the participant and the ADOS administrator. Participants with high-functioning ASD showed significantly longer turn-taking gaps and a greater proportion of pause time, less variability and less synchronous changes in blockwise mean of intensity compared with those with typical development (TD) (p<0.05 corrected). In addition, the ASD group had significantly wider distribution than the TD group in the within-participant variability of blockwise mean of log F0 (p<0.05 corrected). The clinical diagnosis could be discriminated using the speech features with 89% accuracy. The features of turn-taking and pausing were significantly correlated with deficits of ASD in reciprocity (p<0.05 corrected). Additionally, regression analysis provided 1.35 of mean absolute error in the prediction of deficits in reciprocity, to which the synchrony of intensity especially contributed. The findings suggest that considering variance of speech features, interaction and synchrony with conversation partner are critical to characterize atypical features in the conversation of people with ASD.

Keywords:

Social communication – Diagnostic medicine – emotions – Regression analysis – Verbal communication – Autism spectrum disorder – Autism – Speech

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) affects approximately 1% of the general population [1], and currently there is no approved medication for the core symptoms. Individuals with ASD show deficits in social communication and interactions, including nonverbal communicative behaviors (e.g., eye contact, gestures, facial expressions, and speech prosody), as core symptoms [2]. Because diagnosis currently depends mainly on subjective assessment of these behaviors by trained clinicians, developing objective, quantitative, and reproducible assessments for social behavior is expected to improve the accuracy of diagnosis and to promote the further development of novel therapies by accurately detecting time-course changes in the severity of ASD core symptoms [3, 4].

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [5] is the gold-standard diagnostic tool for ASD. Using a semi-structured administration and scoring system, variability in assessment with ADOS is minimized across administrators and subjects. However, because ASD has been considered as static traits, available diagnostic tools, including ADOS, were not originally formulated to be repeatable and to detect changes in symptoms over time. We assume that the quantification of behavioral characteristics based on video footage recorded during easily repeatable activities in ADOS may provide such a valid and reliable quantitative measure for assessing longitudinal changes in core ASD symptoms.

A substantial body of literature describes the characteristics of speech prosody of people with ASD using both objective and subjective measures [6]. Although there are some discrepancies in previous findings [715], a meta-analysis revealed higher mean pitch and wider pitch variance in individuals with ASD [16]. These studies explored the significant differences in mean values between typically developing (TD) and ASD groups. However, Green and Tobin [17] showed that speakers with ASD can be classified into three groups with narrow, wide and typical pitch ranges, pointing out the possibility that comparison of mean value is not adequate. In addition, assessment criteria of ADOS include both exaggerated and monotonous prosody, or speech rate that is too fast or too slow, because these are considered characteristics of ASD.

Some studies have focused on conversation that is assumed to include the characteristics of interaction with other persons. For instance, in experiments measuring the timing of turn-taking, Heeman et al. reported that the turn-taking gap immediately after a question was significantly longer for children with ASD than for those with TD but not after an utterance [18].

In conversation, synchrony—a type of entrainment—is commonly observed in many languages [19]. It can be measured by tracking the similarity of the change trends of the two speakers [20]. Pérez and colleagues revealed that both synchrony and asynchrony play a role in entrainment [21]. In addition, although less reciprocal conversation is one of the core features of ASD in social communication [5], few studies have investigated engagement in conversation by individuals with ASD [22]. Regarding speech features, previous studies have attempted to use acoustic and prosodic features, including statistics related to fundamental frequency (F0) [23], emotional expression [24], and turn-taking features [25, 26], although the conversations examined in these studies were confined to relatively simple interaction in children with ASD.

The aim of our study is to examine whether quantified speech features in recorded footage can be used to detect the behavioral characteristics of individuals with ASD compared with those with TD, and to develop an objective measure that facilitates the detection of changes over time via longitudinal assessments. We analyzed the speech of individuals with ASD and TD from two points of view: First, we hypothesized that the group with ASD would have a wider range of speech features than the TD group even if the distributions of the two groups overlap. Second, we focused on social interaction where deficits in social communication are assumed to occur for people with ASD. Thus, we compared not only the mean values, but also the variance of the distribution of the speech features of the two groups. In addition, analysis of prosody was conducted with long sliding windows to measure the similarity of change trends with conversational partner, to describe the degree of synchrony of the two speakers.

Speech samples were collected from adults with ASD and TD during an activity from ADOS. We investigated speech features including pitch, intensity, speech rate, speaking time, and turn-taking gaps, where the mean and standard deviation (SD) were calculated as basic statistics for quantification. We also carried out a block analysis using an 8s long sliding window to quantify dynamic changes and synchrony with the conversational partner. F-tests were conducted prior to t-tests, comparing the ASD and TD groups to investigate the within-group variance of the two groups. After that, we performed discrimination analysis to classify the individuals into ASD or TD groups using the proposed speech features. The relationships between the speech features and ADOS scores were explored by correlation analysis. Regression analysis was employed to predict ADOS score from the speech features and determine the combination of features best-suited for the prediction.

Methods

Participants

Recruitment, clinical assessments, and data collection were conducted at the University of Tokyo Hospital. Sixty-five adult males with high-functioning ASD participated in the study. They had all been diagnosed with ASD, Asperger’s syndrome, or an unspecified pervasive developmental disorder based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-Revision IV-Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) [27]. A psychiatrist (HY) experienced in developmental disorders made a diagnosis of ASD based on the strict criteria of DSM-IV-TR. A certified psychologist (MK) confirmed the diagnosis using the Japanese version of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [28] and ADOS Module 4 [5]. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale revised III (WAIS) [29] was used to confirm that the participants’ full scale IQs were above 80.

Participants were confirmed not to fulfill the exclusion criteria, viz. 1) primary psychiatric diagnoses other than ASD; 2) instability in symptoms of comorbid mental disorders such as mood disorder or anxiety disorder; 3) history of changes in medication or medication dosage of psychotropics within one month of assessments; 4) currently under treatment with more than two different psychotropics; 5) currently under treatment for comorbid ADHD with atomoxetine or methylphenidate; 6) history of seizures or traumatic brain injury with loss of consciousness for longer than 5 minutes; and, 7) history of alcohol-related disorder, substance abuse or addiction.

In addition, 17 adult Japanese males with a history of typical development (TD) were recruited and matched for age, parental socioeconomic background [30], handedness, and intellectual level. The verbal IQs of these participants were estimated using the Japanese version [31] of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) [32]. While the NART can estimate the verbal IQs of TD individuals, this can be problematic for those with ASD because of imbalanced intellectual abilities often associated with ASD, and well-known discrepancies between subscales of the WAIS. Therefore, the IQs of the participants with ASD were assessed using the WAIS-III. TD participants were screened by trained psychiatrists (HY and KO) for the following exclusion criteria: presence and/or past history of neuropsychiatric disorders using the Structured Clinical Interview [33] and family history of neuropsychiatric disorders in their first-degree relatives.

The Ethical Committee of the University of Tokyo Hospital approved this study (10245). After a complete explanation of the study, participants’ mental capacity to consent was confirmed by a psychiatrist (H.Y. or K.O.), and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

ADOS administration

ADOS module 4 was administered to all participants by a single administrator (HY), who had completed a training course on the use of ADOS for research. Administration was validated by a certified administrator (MK). All ADOS administrations were recorded on video, with scoring the participants with ASD verified by a single certified administrator (MK), thus minimizing inter-administrator and -rater variability. We used the recorded videos for activity 7, ‘Emotions’ in ADOS module 4, for analysis, because participants were required to participate in a conversational interview with the administrator’s fixed questions about various types of emotional experiences, likely to reveal a variety of affective prosody.

From the 65 participants with ASD, two speech samples were excluded because the audio recordings were incomplete. In addition, one participant with ASD withdrew consent and his data were then excluded. In total we analyzed 79 recordings from 62 participants with ASD and 17 individuals with TD.

Recording paradigm

A pair of wireless lavalier microphones (ECM-AW4, Sony Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) was attached to the collars of the participant and the ADOS administrator, to record their conversation with a favorable signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), without disrupting their natural speech like headset microphones sometimes do. The sampling frequency was 48 or 44.1 kHz, downsampled to 16 kHz after recording and with quantization with 16-bit precision.

Speech feature processing

We extracted two types of speech features related to prosody and voice activity. Preprocessing for both features involved manual extraction of the inter pausal units (IPUs) of each participant and administrator. IPUs were defined as speech intervals divided by silence longer than 200 ms and not included within a word.

Prosodic features

Prosodic features analyzed in this study included the log F0, intensity, and speech rate. Both the log F0 and intensity were computed using Praat [34], with a frame shift of 10 ms. This was only done for spoken parts of the detected IPUs of each participant or administrator. Overlaps in participant and administrator speech were excluded from the analysis. For robust F0 extraction, we followed the method described in [35]. We first applied Praat pitch detection with a fixed pitch floor and ceiling for all participants. The pitch ceiling and floor were set at 32 and 200 Hz, respectively, based on observation of the speech data. For the distribution of the extracted pitch for each participant, we applied Praat pitch detection again with the pitch floor set to 0.75 times the lower quantile and the pitch ceiling set to double the upper quantile. The pitch floor and ceiling were manually tuned for only two participants. In the intensity analysis, the frame length was 32 ms.

Finally, the IPUs were transcribed as phoneme sequences using their temporal boundary information by the speech recognition engine Julius. A Japanese monophone Hidden Markov Model (HMM) distributed in the Julius dictation kit (version 4.0) [36] was used as an acoustic model. The number of morae in each IPU was obtained from the transcription to calculate the speech rate. In this study, speech rate was calculated by dividing the total number of morae by total duration of speech excluding silence, so that the pause and ‘rate of articulation’ were analyzed separately. Note that the mora was used as the unit of calculation for speech rate instead of the syllable, because morae are the rhythmic units in Japanese.

Let f0[i] and J[i] be the value of F0 (in Hz) and intensity (in decibels) of a participant at the i th frame. We calculated the F0 and intensity only within IPU, and F0 was extracted only at the voiced frame. We then calculated the overall and blockwise statistics.

1) Overall session features

For the overall session statistics, we calculated the mean and SD for F0 and intensity over the whole session. The mean of the intensity was excluded since it could depend on the unknown distance between the microphone and the speaker. The mean of log F0, mean of intensity, SD of log F0, and SD of intensity were extracted as follows:

Where ΓV and Γ are the set of frame indices when F0 and intensity were calculated, respectively.

2) Blockwise Features

Fig 1 shows the flow for calculation of blockwise statistics from frame features. To analyze the long duration changes and synchrony of the pitch and intensity, we performed a block analysis using the time-aligned moving average (TAMA) method [37]. We defined x[i] as a value of the log F0 or intensity at the ith frame. The blockwise mean value of kth block is defined by:

Fig. 1. Calculation of frame and block statistics of prosodic features.
Calculation of frame and block statistics of prosodic features.
where ΓB[k] is a set of frame numbers included in the kth block. It is defined by:
where n is the number of frames included in a block. Note that #ΓB[k] equals the numbers of the frames included in IPUs in the case of intensity, whereas #ΓB[k] is the number of voiced frames in that block in the case of F0.

The SD of the blockwise mean of the log F0 or intensity is similarly obtained by:

where N and mxB are the number of blocks that comprise the whole session and the mean value of xB[k] over the whole session, respectively.

The normalized correlation coefficient was calculated using Eq 9. This value represents the similarity of the prosodic features of a participant and administrator in the activity

where xB[k] and yB[k] are the blockwise mean of a prosodic feature of a participant and an administrator at kth block.

The block length and step were set to 16s and 8s, respectively, in this study. Overlapping speech of an administrator and participant was excluded from the analysis. We then obtained the SD of the average intensity and pitch in every block (blockwise mean) of a participant, and the correlation coefficient for an administrator and participant.

Voice activity features

Voice activity features were obtained from the detected IPUs. Speaking time was defined as the duration of the IPU in seconds. Fig 2 shows the definition of a turn-taking gap, viz. the time from the end of the administrator’s utterance to the beginning of the participant’s next utterance. When the participant started to speak before the administrator had finished speaking, the turn-taking time has a negative value. We did not include the backchannels, which are defined as the utterances inserted into the interlocutor’s utterances without interrupting in many literatures [38], into a turn based on the study by Sato et al. [39]. For the calculation of turn-taking gaps, we omitted utterances that overlapped entirely with the administrator’s utterance, regarding them as backchannels or failures of turn-taking in which the participant continued to speak after the administrator tried to take a turn.

Fig. 2. IPUs and turn-taking gaps.
IPUs and turn-taking gaps.

We calculated the pause-to-turn ratio as the ratio of the total pause duration to the total speaking-turn duration in a session. For automatic processing, we simply defined the speaking turn as the time segment from the beginning of an utterance longer than the threshold to the end of the utterance before the start of an utterance from the other speaker that exceeded the threshold. We heuristically set 0.5s as the threshold, which means that utterances shorter than 0.5 seconds, such as backchannels, did not count as turns. The pause-to-turn ratio was large when a participant maintained a long silence after obtaining a turn.

Statistical analysis

To assess the normality of the distribution of the speech features of the ASD and TD group, we conducted Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests on each feature of each group. Based on the results which showed the non-normality in the distribution of the mean and SD of turn-taking gap and the turn-to-pause ratio (p < 0.05), we took the logarithm transform on the three features, referring to the findings that the logarithm of the duration of the gaps and overlaps of turn takings have a normal distribution [40] [41]. For only the mean of turn-taking gap, we added a constant to all samples such that the minimum value of the feature was equal to 1 prior to the logarithm transform for non-negativity. We confirmed that Kolmogorov-Smirnov test showed no significance on these logarithmically transformed features.

Difference in the mean value of each feature between the ASD and TD groups was compared by t-test. Prior to the t-tests, we conducted F-tests to compare the variance because the features vary among individuals with ASD. Statistical significance for all analyses was set at p < 0.05 after adjustment with the Benjamini-Hochberg (B-H) multiple comparisons procedure [42].

As an exploratory analysis, we applied a support vector machine (SVM) to discriminate the two groups of speakers using the e1071 [43] package in the statistical environment R. We used linear kernel because it provided the best performance in our experiment. We evaluated the classifier by one-leave-out cross-validation. Because the sample sizes of the two groups were imbalanced, we weighted the objective function used in the training of the SVM according to the inverse number of the sample size of each class to avoid the undervaluation of the false negative. We tested three feature sets which consisted of all 13 speech features (Setting D1), only features where results of F- or t-tests showed a significant diagnostic difference (Setting D2), and the best combination of features selected based on accuracy from all possible 8191 combinations (Setting D3).

The number of the possible combination (8191) represents the number of all cases where each of 13 speech features were used or not (213 − 1). In the evaluation of the D3 setting, we used leave-one-out cross-validation to avoid overfitting. In details, the accuracy of each combinations of speech features (Combination i) was evaluated by the following equation:

where bk takes 1 or 0 when the classification is accurate or not respectively, in the test for the kth participant’s data by the classifier trained using all the other participants’ data (i.e., all 78 participants except for the kth one). NASD+TD represents the total number of the participants of the ASD and TD groups (79 in this study).

The correlational analyses of the extracted speech features and ADOS scores were conducted after omitting the features which had no significant differences in the t- and F-tests comparing the two groups. We focused on the score of the reciprocity in the ADOS score because it was assumed to reflect the attributes of the interaction between the participants and administrator. We also analyzed the correlation between the speech features and the scores of communication and repetitive domains of ADOS as references.

In addition to the above calculations, we considered the possibility that the speech features of the participants with ASD may have a wider distribution than those with TD. High ADOS scores arise when individuals’ speech features are very great or small. Thus, in this study, we applied a transformation to speech features that showed significance in the F-test and no significance in the t-test, using the following equation to compute the distances from the mean of the speech features in the TD group:

where xi and mTD are the value of a speech feature of ith participant and the mean value of the speech feature in the TD group, respectively. These transformed speech features were used in the correlation analysis instead of the raw values. We used a permutation test for the SD of blockwise mean of log F0, which did not have a normal distribution (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, p < 0.05).

In addition to the above analyses and experiment, we conducted a prediction experiment using the SVR included in the e1071 package in R with linear kernel. The following three feature sets were tested:

  • All 13 features (Setting R1)

  • The features included in Setting D2 that had a significant correlation with each ADOS score in the correlation analysis (Setting R2)

  • The features included in Setting D3 in the discrimination analysis (Setting R3)

  • The combination of speech features when root mean square error (RMSE) was the smallest (Setting R4).

The regressors in these four settings were evaluated based on the correlation coefficient of the predicted and rated ADOS score, RMSE, and mean absolute error (MAE), using a leave-one-out correlation coefficient. Similar to the Setting D3 in the discrimination analysis, the possible 8191 combination was evaluated in Setting R4. Carrying out one-leave-out cross-validation in the same way, the correlation coefficients, MAE, and RMSE were obtained by averaging the values when the data a single participant was tested from the regressor trained by the data of the other participants, respectively.

Results

Background information

There was no significant difference in age, verbal IQ, or parental socio-economic status (SES) between the participants with ASD and those with TD (p > 0.05) (Table 1). The individuals with ASD had significantly lower self SES compared with the participants with TD (p = 4.67 × 10−14).

Tab. 1. Demographic characteristics of participants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and those with typical development (TD).
Demographic characteristics of participants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and those with typical development (TD).

Comparisons of groups with and without ASD (Table 2)

Tab. 2. Means and SDs of speech features of each group and the adjusted p values.
Means and SDs of speech features of each group and the adjusted p values.

Fig 3 shows two examples of the time series of the mean intensity of the two speakers. In the upper case, the intensity values sometimes move in opposite directions and at other times together. In the middle case, the intensity of the participant moves inversely to that of the administrator. The lower case, in contrast, shows the intensity of the participant increases and decreases together with the administrator’s value.

Fig. 3. Examples of time series of the blockwise mean of the intensity of the utterances of a participant with ASD and administrator (upper and middle panel) and the utterances of a participant with TD and administrator (lower panel).
Examples of time series of the blockwise mean of the intensity of the utterances of a participant with ASD and administrator (upper and middle panel) and the utterances of a participant with TD and administrator (lower panel).
The correlation coefficient of the features between the two speakers had a close-to-zero value (0.018) in the upper case, a negative value (-0.26) in the middle case, and a positive value (0.65) in the lower case.

Table 2 shows the means and SDs of the two groups and the p-values for the t- and F-tests. Fig 4 shows the histograms of the log mean of turn-taking gap, the SD of blockwise mean of intensity, and the SD of blockwise mean of log F0. Both t- and F-tests showed significant differences between the ASD and TD group in the log mean of turn-taking gap, and the log SD of turn-taking gap (adjusted p<0.05) (Table 2, Fig 4). For each of these three features, the means and SDs were significantly greater in participants with ASD than those with TD. Fig 4 shows the histograms of speech features that showed significance in t-and/or F- tests.

Fig. 4. Histograms of (a) SD of blockwise mean of log F0, (b) SD of blockwise mean of intensity, (c) correlation coefficient of blockwise mean of intensity, (d) log mean of turn-taking gap, (e) log SD of turn-taking gap, and (f) log pause-to-turn ratio.
Histograms of (a) SD of blockwise mean of log <i>F</i><sub>0</sub>, (b) SD of blockwise mean of intensity, (c) correlation coefficient of blockwise mean of intensity, (d) log mean of turn-taking gap, (e) log SD of turn-taking gap, and (f) log pause-to-turn ratio.
Nineteen out of 62 individuals with ASD (30.1%) had a mean turn-taking gap greater than 0.8s, whereas none of the 17 participants with TD had such a long mean gap. The pause-to-turn ratio of 17 individuals with ASD (27.4%) was greater than 0.1 whereas that of the individuals with TD was distributed in the range 0–0.1. Fourteen participants (22.6%) with ASD had negative correlation coefficients for intensity, whereas only one (5.9%) participant with TD had that. The absolute value of the correlation coefficient was less than 0.1 in 13 individuals with ASD (20.1%) and two with TD (11.8%).

Regarding the SD and correlation coefficient of blockwise mean of intensity, the pause-to-turn ratio, the t-test found significant differences between ASD and TD groups, although the F-test found no significant difference. The SD of blockwise mean of intensity was significantly smaller in the individuals with ASD (M = 2.6, SD = 1.1) compared with those with TD (M = 3.5, SD = 0.77) (Fig 4b), although the SDs of intensity within the activity were not significantly different. A small proportion of the ASD group (6.5%) with high SD of blockwise mean of intensity (> 0.2), murmured or whispered the question asked by the administrator to themselves while they were thinking, e.g. Administrator: “What do you think is scary?” Participant: “Scary…”). These quiet repetitions expanded the range of intensity.

The correlation coefficient of the intensity of the individuals with ASD (M = 0.18, SD = 0.24) was also significantly smaller than those with TD (M = 0.34, SD = 0.20) (Fig 4c). The pause-to-turn ratio of those of ASD was significantly larger than that of ASD. The F-test for the blockwise mean of F0 showed a significant difference, even though no significant difference was observed in the t-test.

All other speech features showed no significant differences in either t- or F-tests. Although there was no significant difference in the features other than the SD of blockwise mean of log F0, the log mean and SD of turn-taking gap, and the pause-to-turn ratio in F-tests, the variance of all features except the speech rate and correlation coefficient of log F0 were greater in the individuals with ASD than those with TD.

Discrimination of ASD and TD groups (Table 3)

Tab. 3. Confusion matrix of ASD and TD discrimination.
Confusion matrix of ASD and TD discrimination.

Table 3 shows the confusion matrix of the discrimination analyses in Settings D1, D2 and D3. Table 4 gives the accuracy and F-measure in each setting. Setting D3, with the best set of features, achieved 88.6% accuracy and 92.3% F-measure. The accuracy in Setting D2, which used the speech features selected based on t- and F-tests, did not exceed that of Setting D1. The accuracy of Setting D2 was 11.4% points below that of Setting D3.

Tab. 4. Percentage of accuracy and F-measure in the discrimination analysis.
Percentage of accuracy and F-measure in the discrimination analysis.

Table 5 lists the selected features in Setting D3, and the accuracy and F-measure when each feature was used alone for the classification using SVM. The log mean turn-taking gap provided the highest accuracy when it was used alone for the classification. The log mean of turn-taking gap and the SD of blockwise mean of intensity were included in at least the top 20 sets with accuracy, from all 8191 combinations.

Tab. 5. Selected speech features in Setting D3 and the percentage of accuracy and F-measure when each feature was used alone for classification.
Selected speech features in Setting D3 and the percentage of accuracy and F-measure when each feature was used alone for classification.

Correlations between speech features and ADOS score

Table 6 shows Pearson’s correlation coefficients between ADOS scores and the speech features that showed significant differences in the F- or t-tests for individuals with ASD and TD (S1 Fig). The reciprocity scores showed significant correlations with the log mean of turn-taking gap (r = 0.44), the log of SD of turn-taking gap (r = 0.35) and the log of pause-to-turn ratio (r = 0.41) (adjusted p<0.01). In contrast, the communication and repetitive scores had no significant correlation with any speech features. Fig 5 shows scatter plots representing relationships of the log mean of turn-taking gap (left), log of SD of turn-taking gap (middle), and log of pause-to-turn ratio (right) to reciprocity score in the participants with ASD.

Tab. 6. Correlation coefficients (r) between the speech features and ADOS scores and adjusted p-values of correlation test.
Correlation coefficients (r) between the speech features and ADOS scores and adjusted p-values of correlation test.
Fig. 5. Scatter plots between the features that have significant correlations with the reciprocity score.
Scatter plots between the features that have significant correlations with the reciprocity score.
The black dotted line represents a regression line. The blue solid and dashed lines represent the mean and mean ± SD of the feature among the participants with TD, respectively. A positive correlation was observed between the log of pause-to-turn ratio and reciprocity score of the individuals with ASD. At the same time, the distributions of the ASD and TD groups overlapped in the area between −5 and −2. Similar overlaps were observed in mean and log of SD of turn-taking gap.

Although the SD of blockwise mean of intensity had no significant correlation with reciprocity, the scatter plot had a U- or V-shaped distribution. The reciprocity score has a low value when the feature is around the mean value of the TD. The reciprocity score increases as the features depart from the mean value of TD. The features of the participants with ASD were intensively distributed within the lower values: the features of 53 participants with ASD (85.5%) were lower than the mean for the TD group. It is also notable that the individuals whose value was distant from the average of TD received a relatively low score (e.g. 6). In the following regression analysis, the SD of blockwise mean intensity was used without converting because it did not improve the result.

Regression analysis between speech features and ADOS score

Table 7 shows the correlation coefficients for the ADOS scores rated clinically and those predicted by the regressor, the MAE and RMSE of the predicted scores. The correlation of reciprocity scores was improved in Setting R2 and R3 compared with Setting R1. In Settings R1, R3 and R4, the correlation between the predicted and rated reciprocity score was highest among three score domains.

Tab. 7. Correlation coefficients (r) between rated and predicted ADOS scores, MAE and RMSE of predicted ADOS scores.
Correlation coefficients (r) between rated and predicted ADOS scores, MAE and RMSE of predicted ADOS scores.

Table 8 shows the selected speech features in Setting R4. The speech features selected for the prediction of communication and repetitive score was respectively involved in the selected features for the prediction of reciprocity. Log of mean of speaking time gave the highest correlation between rated and predicted reciprocity score when it was used alone. Using only the correlation coefficient of blockwise mean of intensity alone, the predicted reciprocity score did not yield a correlation with the rated score. However, the correlation between rated and predicted score was the third most degraded (0.47 from 0.58) when the feature was excluded from the best feature set.

Tab. 8. Selected features in Setting R4 and the correlation coefficients between the rated and predicted ADOS scores when each feature was used alone (Corr. Single) and omitted from the best set (Corr. Omitted).
Selected features in Setting R4 and the correlation coefficients between the rated and predicted ADOS scores when each feature was used alone (Corr. Single) and omitted from the best set (Corr. Omitted).

Fig 6 shows the scatter plot of each category of ADOS scores predicted by SVR in Setting R4 and rated by the administrator. For the repetitive score, all of the predicted scores were in the narrow range between 0.95 and 3.5 although the rated scores ranged from 0 to 6.

Fig. 6. Scatterplots of the (a) reciprocity, (b) communication and (c) repetitive scores that were predicted in Setting R4 and rated by the administrator.
Scatterplots of the (a) reciprocity, (b) communication and (c) repetitive scores that were predicted in Setting R4 and rated by the administrator.

Discussion

The current study showed that individuals with ASD: (1) spoke with a uniform volume regardless of topics; (2) used wide variations in pitch depending on circumstances; (3) were less synchronous with interlocutors in volume of voice; and (4) demonstrated longer pauses between and within turns (Table 2, Fig 4). It was further noted that: (5) the length of pauses correlated with the reciprocity score in ADOS (Fig 5); and (6) the discrimination and regression analysis could successfully detect ASD individuals and predict the reciprocity score (Tables 37, Fig 6).

Comparison of groups with and without ASD

Variance of within-participant blockwise statistics

The SD of blockwise mean of intensity was significantly smaller in the speakers with ASD compared with those with TD, although the SD of intensity within a whole session showed no significant difference between the two groups. The participants with TD had 8s blocks with both low and high intensity. This indicates that people with TD speak more frequently with loud or quiet voices depending on circumstances. In contrast, the individuals with ASD in our study spoke with a similar volume in each block.

For the SD of blockwise mean of log F0, the variance among speakers was significantly greater in the ASD group than the TD group in spite of no significant difference for within-group means. The within-group SD of this feature was 82.6% larger in the ASD group compared with the TD group. The SD of log F0 within a session had a similar (but not significant) tendency where the within-group variance was wider in the ASD group than in the TD group. These results are consistent with the finding by [17], which subdivided children with ASD into three groups with narrow, wide and typical ranges of F0. Nevertheless, it is necessary to note that unlike that study, the SD was calculated among blocks in this study. The large SDs of blockwise mean of log F0 are assumed to represent the large change in pitch among various contexts that appeared in the interaction with the conversational partner. From this point of view, the participants with ASD could be subdivided into groups that change their pitch in a wider, narrower, or typical range in different blocks. While some participants with ASD spoke in almost the same pitch in each block throughout the session, other participants changed pitch among blocks. The conflicting findings about the variance of F0 among many previous researchers could be caused by the large distribution of the feature across individuals with ASD. Fusaroli et al. showed significantly wider F0 variance in the population with ASD using meta-analysis, however, the speech data sets and the methods for measuring F0 variance were diverse among studies, as the authors noted.

Asynchrony of prosodic features

The TD group had a higher correlation coefficient of the blockwise mean of intensity with their partner than the ASD group. A greater proportion of the participants with ASD had a negative correlation coefficient than those with TD. The results indicate that speakers with TD and their interlocutors spoke more synchronously than those with ASD and their partner. More participants with ASD spoke with quiet voices when the administrator spoke loudly, and vice versa. Synchrony is one of the components of entrainment related to social aspects such as speaker engagement [44]. As Gupta [22] showed, the degree of engagement will be different depending on circumstances in people with ASD as well. In this study, because the recorded conversations were limited to semi-structured interviews, the strength of synchrony could be compared among participants. Lower synchrony is possibly a characteristic of the conversation of people with ASD. However, the negative correlation coefficient of the acoustic features does not seem to simply mean disengagement: Pérez et al. showed that negative correlation coefficients also represent speaker engagement [21]. Further investigation should be conducted to identify the meaning of negative synchrony observed in some of the speakers with ASD.

Long silence

The individuals with ASD had significantly longer turn-taking gaps than those with TD. This finding was consistent with the results of Heeman et al. [18] who described long turn-taking gaps in children with ASD following a question, as in the ADOS activity used in the current study that consisted of questions and answers about emotional experiences. Furthermore, looking at each turn-taking gap within individuals, eight participants with ASD (12.9%) had one or more extended turn-taking gaps (> 5 s), which was not observed in any of the TD group. Speakers with TD inserted fillers, such as ‘um’ and ‘uh’ in English, to stall for time and to keep their turn in a dialogue [45]. It is possible that individuals with ASD exhibiting long turn-taking gaps did not use fillers. The commonly-used English filler ‘um’ was observed significantly less frequently in children with ASD than those with TD, as reported by Clark et al. [46]. They proposed that ‘um’ is a signal of a lengthy upcoming delay common to speakers and listeners, based on comparison between ‘um’ and ‘uh’ by measuring listener comprehension [46]. In fact the delay after ‘um’ is longer than ‘uh’ but not as long as after ‘uːm’ [46]. Japanese fillers, their frequency of use and function, need to be investigated in future studies.

Individuals with ASD had significantly higher pause-to-turn ratios than those with TD. The ratio of the ASD group was 11% on average, three times that of the TD group. Morett et al. showed an elevated pause-to-turn ratio in children with ASD [47], and Thurber and Tager-Flusberg showed more frequent ungrammatical pauses in adolescents with ASD [48]. These findings about frequency were based on analysis of narrative production. In contrast, using interaction in ADOS administrations, Bone et al. found that intra-turn pauses correlated with ADOS scores [17]. It is considered from these findings that more frequent pauses were produced, and that silences were longer after taking a turn. People with ASD may allow more silence instead of inserting fillers or other utterances to keep their turn.

Turn-taking gaps of participants in the TD group were also characterized by a high frequency of negative gaps accompanying the responses initiated before the administrator’s utterance ended. The mean relative frequency of negative turn-taking gaps was 61.7% in the TD group, while that frequency was 39.4% in ASD group. It is assumed that the negative turn-taking gaps also reduced the within-participant mean of turn-taking gaps. A previous multilingual comparison revealed that speakers generally minimize overlaps and turn-taking gaps, but Japanese speakers favor shorter gaps including negative ones [49]. These short gaps are conceivably related to the ability to predict turn ends, which can be predicted from lexico-syntactic and prosodic cues [50].

Discrimination analysis

Discrimination analysis showed that selecting features revealed to be significantly different in t- or F-test (Setting D2) did not improve the accuracy of the discrimination. Selecting the best set of features provided 11.4-point better accuracy compared with Setting D2 (Table 4). However, the log mean of turn-taking gap and SD of blockwise mean of intensity, which showed significant differences in t-tests, greatly contributed to discrimination in Setting D3 (Table 5).

Focusing on a single speech feature, there was a significant overlap between the ASD and TD groups as shown in Fig 4. In addition, the combination of selected multiple speech features provided higher accuracy than when using one of them alone. Therefore, it is suggested that the conversational strategies of individuals with ASD can be characterized by a combination of multiple speech features. At the same time, it is possible that one or more speech features that serve as a clue for discrimination will differ in individuals or subgroups with ASD.

Correlation between speech features and ADOS scores

Three speech features related to turn-taking and pausing had moderate correlations with the reciprocity score in ADOS (Table 6 and Fig 5). In ADOS, reciprocity was assessed by integrating speech and visual information used during social interaction, such as eye contact, facial expression and gesture. However, these aspects were not directly measured in this study. Strategies of turn-taking and pausing, together with physical behavior, reflect the mutual reciprocity between participant and administrator, and the characteristics of how the participant establishes and maintains communication. As shown in Fig 5, the speech features of individuals with ASD who had low reciprocity scores were in almost the same range as the TD group. Speech features may not have decreased in the same proportion even in the case when the reciprocity score was close to zero.

In addition, for the SD of blockwise mean of intensity, individuals with features close to the mean of the TD group had low reciprocity scores, whereas those with features distant from the mean of TD had wide ranging scores (from 6 to 14). These observations indicate that correlation analysis is not necessarily sufficient to describe the relationship between speech features and ADOS scores. The prediction of ADOS score was degraded by the conversion of the blockwise mean of intensity using Eq (11), information about whether the feature played a role in the prediction.

Regression analysis between speech features and ADOS score

The prediction of reciprocity scores using the features that showed a significant correlation with the ADOS (R2) score gave a better result than when all 13 features (R1), or the best features selected for the discrimination (R3), were used. Although Setting R3 was better than Setting R2, the correlation between predicted and actual scores was higher in Setting R4 than Setting R3 (Table 7). This means that the best combination of features for the classification does not provide the best performance in the regression.

In the selected features in Setting R4, the mean of speaking time gave the best prediction when it was used alone. However, it was the poorest predictor when the correlation-coefficient of blockwise mean intensity was omitted from the best set of features. This indicates that the correlation-coefficient of blockwise mean intensity improves the prediction performance when combined with other features.

As shown in Fig 6(a), two individuals with low actual reciprocity scores (3–5) received predictions that were too high, in error. This may have been because of the lack of speech samples from individuals with a low reciprocity score. As the performance of prediction of ADOS repetitive score was not sufficient, the speech features are considered not to depend on the characteristics related to repetitive score.

The mean of speaking time and correlation coefficient were selected for the prediction of both reciprocity and communication score. In contrast, speech rate was selected for communication score, not for reciprocity. The result relates to the criteria for communication scores in the ADOS protocol which includes assessment of speech rate.

Potential limitations

The study has several limitations and methodological considerations. First, the number of participants with TD was small. Although the current findings were robust so that significance was maintained after correction for multiple comparisons, the detection of speech features related to ASD diagnosis could be limited. Second, race, sex, and age of the participants were limited to Japanese adult males. While this uniformity may have enhanced detection of speech features by controlling potential confounding effects of the participant characteristics, the findings should be applied to other populations with caution. Third, the estimation of IQs in TD and ASD groups was made in different methods. As IQ scores did not significantly correlated with the quantified speech features in the ASD or TD groups (p>0.23), it is less likely that the potential bias in the IQ assessment induced by the different methods had significant influences on the findings regarding differences in speech features between ASD and TD groups.

Conclusions

In this study, conversation arising from a semi-structured interview was investigated to quantify the speech characteristics of people with ASD. We analyzed the speech features related to prosody, turn-taking, pausing, and synchrony of the conversation. Individuals with ASD spoke with a wide variety of pitch control strategies. The results also showed that individuals with ASD used similar volume regardless of the flow of conversation and with less synchronization with conversation partners, in comparison to those with TD. As regards to the timing in conversation, individuals with ASD responded after longer silence and more pauses after taking their turns. The discrimination analysis achieved high accuracy by combining the speech features related to the variance of prosody, duration of pause within speech, and duration of turn-taking gaps. The SD of blockwise mean of intensity contributed most to the discrimination.

The features of turn-taking and pausing correlated significantly with deficits associated with ASD in reciprocity. However, a simple correlation may not sufficiently describe the relationship between the speech features and deficits of ASD. Difficulties in reciprocity were effectively predicted using a set of features of SD and synchrony of intensity, mean of log duration of pause within speech, duration of turn-taking gap and log of pause-to-turn ratio. Synchrony of intensity contributed to the prediction of features of ASD even though it did not show a significant correlation with the ADOS score alone.

Supporting information

S1 Fig [r]
Scatter plots representing the all calculated correlations between each speech feature and subscales of ADOS a) reciprocity, b) communication, and c) repetitive.


Zdroje

1. Baio J, Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders: Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 Sites, United States, 2008, MMWR Surveill Summ. 2012;61(3):1–19. 22456193

2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5 5th edn, American Psychiatric Publishing: Washington, D.C. 2013.

3. Owada K., Kojima M., Yassin W., Kuroda M., Kawakubo Y., Kuwabara H., et al. Computer-analyzed facial expression as a surrogate marker for autism spectrum social core symptoms. PLoS One, 2008;13(1):e0190442.

4. Owada K, Okada T, Munesue T, Kuroda M, Fujioka T, Uno Y, Matsumoto K, Kuwabara H, Mori D, Okamoto Y, Yoshimura Y, Kawakubo Y, Arioka Y, Kojima M, Yuhi T, Yassin W, Kushima I, Benner S, Ogawa N, Kawano N, Eriguchi Y, Uemura Y, Yamamoto M, Kano Y, Kasai K, Higashida H, Ozaki N, Kosaka H, Yamasue H. Quantitative facial expression analysis revealed the efficacy and time course of oxytocin in autism. Brain. 2019; May 16.

5. Lord C, Rutter M, Goode S, Heemsberge J, Mawhood L, et al. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: a Standardized Observation of Communicative and Social Behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1989;19(2):185–212. doi: 10.1007/bf02211841 2745388

6. McCann J, Peppé S, Prosody in Autism Spectrum Disorders: a Critical Review, International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2003;38(4):325–350. doi: 10.1080/1368282031000154204 14578051

7. Sharda M, Subhadra TP, Sahay S, Nagaraja C, Singh L, Mishra R Singh NC. Sounds of Melody—Pitch Patterns of Speech in Autism. Neuroscience letters. 2010; 478(1):42–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.066 20447444

8. Bonneh SY, Levanon Y, Dean-Pardo O, Lossos L, Adini Y, Abnormal Speech Spectrum and Increased Pitch Variability in Young Autistic Children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2011;4:237. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00237 21267429

9. Nadig A, Shaw H, Acoustic and Perceptual Measurement of Expressive Prosody in High-functioning Autism: Increased Pitch Range and What It Means to Listeners. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42(4): 499–511. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1264-3 21528425

10. Filipe MG, Frota S, Castro SL, Vicente SG. Atypical Prosody in Asperger Syndrome: Perceptual and Acoustic Measurements. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;44(8):1972–1981. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2073-2 24590408

11. Kaland C, Krahmer E, Swerts M. Contrastive Intonation in Autism: The Effect of Speaker-and Listener-perspective. Thirteenth Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association. 2012.

12. Nakai Y, Takashima R, Takiguchi T, Takada S, Speech Intonation in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain and Development. 2014;36(6):516–522. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2013.07.006 23973369

13. Tanaka H, Sakti SH, Neubig G, Toda T, Nakamura S. Linguistic and Acoustic Features for Automatic Identification of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Children’s Narrative, Proceedings of the ACL2014 Workshop on Computational Linguistics and Clinical Psychology. 2014;88–96.

14. Scharfstein LA, Beidel DC, Sims VK, Finnell LR. Social Skills Deficits and Vocal Characteristics of Children with Social Phobia or Asperger’s Disorder: A Comparative Study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39(6):865–875. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9498-2 21399935

15. Grossman RB, Bemis RH, Skwerer DP, Tager-Flusberg H. Lexical and Affective Prosody in Children with High-functioning Autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2010;53(3):778–793. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0127) 20530388

16. Fusaroli R, Lambrechts A., Bang D, Bowler DM, Gaigg SB. Is Voice a Marker for Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Autism Research. 2017;10(3):384–407. doi: 10.1002/aur.1678 27501063

17. Green H, Tobin Y, Prosodic Analysis is Difficult … But Worth It: A Study in High Functioning Autism, Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2009;11(4):308–315.

18. Heeman P, Lunsford R, Selfridge E, Black L, van Santen L, Autism and Interactional Aspects of Dialogue, Proceedings of 11th Annual Meeting of the Special Interest Group on Discourse and Dialogue. 2010;249–252.

19. Levitan R, Beňuš Š, Gravano A, Hirschberg J. Acoustic-Prosodic Entrainment in Slovak, Spanish, English and Chinese: A Cross-linguistic Comparison. Proceedings of the 16th Annual Meeting of the Special Interest Group on Discourse and Dialogue. 2015:325–334.

20. Kousidis S, Dorran D, McDonnell C, Coyle E. Times Series Analysis of Acoustic Feature Convergence in Human Dialogues. Proceedings of Interspeech. 2008.

21. Pérez JM, Gálvez RH, and Gravano A. Disentrainment May Be a Positive Thing: A Novel Measure of Unsigned Acoustic-Prosodic Synchrony, and Its Relation to Speaker Engagement. Proceedings of Interspeech 2016. 2016;1270–1274.

22. Gupta R, Bone D, Lee S, Narayanan S. Analysis of Engagement Behavior in Children During Dyadic Interactions Using Prosodic Cues. Computer Speech amd Language. 2016;37:47–66.

23. Kakihara Y, Takiguchi T, Ariki Y, Nakai Y Takada S. Investigation of Classification Using Pitch Features for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and Typically Developing Children. American Journal of Signal Processing. 2015;5(1):1–5.

24. Ringeval F, Marchi E, Grossard C, Xavier J, Chetouani M, Cohen D, Schuller B. Automatic Analysis of Typicaland Atypical Encoding of Spontaneous Emotion in the Voice of Children. Proceedings of Interspeech 2016. 2016;1210–1204.

25. Bone D, Lee CC, Black MP, Williams ME, Lee S, Levitt P, Narayanan S. The Psychologist as an Interlocutor in Autism Spectrum Disorder Assessment: Insights from a Study of Spontaneous Prosody. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2014;57(4):1162–1177. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-S-13-0062 24686340

26. Bone D, Bishop S, Gupta R, Lee S, Narayanan S. Acoustic-Prosodic and Turn-Taking Features in Interactions with Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Proceedings of Interspeech 2016;1185–1189.

27. American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR, 4th edn, American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, 2000.

28. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1994;24:659–685. doi: 10.1007/bf02172145 7814313

29. Wechsler D, Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. Psychological Corp.: New York, 1981.

30. Hollingshead AB. Two factor index of social position. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University, Dept. of Sociology; 1957.

31. Matsuoka K, Uno M, Kasai K, Koyama K, Kim Y. Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease using Japanese ideographic script (Kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;60(3):332–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01510.x 16732750

32. Nelson HE. The National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson; 1982.

33. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBM. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition. (SCID-I/NP). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York, 2002.

34. Boersma P. Accurate Short-Term Analysis of the Fundamental Frequency and the Harmonics-to-noise Ratio of a Sampled Sound. Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences. 1993;17(1193):97–110.

35. De Looze C Hirst DJ. Detecting Changes in Key and Range for the Automatic Modelling and Coding of Intonation. Proceeding of Speech Prosody, 2008.

36. Lee A. Kawahara T. Recent Development of Open-Source Speech Recognition Engine Julius. Proceedings: APSIPA ASC 2009: Asia-Pacific Signal and Information Processing Association, 2009 Annual Summit and Conference. 2009;131–137.

37. Levitan R. Hirschberg J. Measuring Acoustic-Prosodic Entrainment with Respect to Multiple Levels and Dimensions. Proceedings of Interspeech 2011, 2011;3081–3084.

38. Heldner M, Edlund J. Pauses, Gaps and Overlaps in Conversations. Journal of Phonetics. 2010;38(4):555–568.

39. Sato R, Higashinaka R, Tamoto M, Nakano M, Aikawa K. Learning Decision Trees to Determine Turn-Taking by Spoken Dialogue Systems. Proceedings of 7th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing. 2002;861–864.

40. Weilhammer K, Rabold S. Durational Aspecats in Turn Taking. Proceedings of the International Conference of Phonetic Sciences. 2003.

41. Ten Bosch L, Oostdijk N, De Ruiter JP. Durational Aspects of Turn-Taking in Spontaneous Face-to-Face and Telephone Dialogues. Proceedings of International Conference on Text, Speech and Dialogue. 2004; 563–570.

42. Benjamini Y. Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: a Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 1995;57(1):289–300.

43. Dimitriadou E, Hornik K, Leisch F, Meyer D, Weingessel A, e1071: Misc Functions of the Department of Statistics (e1071). TU Wien. R package version 1. 2011;6–7.

44. Lunsford R, Heeman P, Black LM, van Santen JP. Autism and the Use of Fillers: Differences Between ‘Um’ and ‘Uh’. DiSS-LPSS Joint Workshop, 2010;107–110.

45. Tree JEF. Listeners’ Uses Ofum Anduh in Speech Comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 2001;29(2)320–326. doi: 10.3758/bf03194926 11352215

46. Clark HH, Tree JEF. Using Uh and Um in Spontaneous Speaking. Cognition, 2002;84(1): 73–111. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00017-3 12062148

47. Morett LM, O’Hearn K, Luna B, Ghuman AS. Altered Gesture and Speech Production in ASD Detract from In-person Communicative Quality. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2016;46(3):998–1012. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2645-9 26520147

48. Thurber C, Tager-Flusberg H. Pauses in the Narratives Produced by Autistic, Mentally Retarded, and Normal Children as an Index of Cognitive Demand. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1993;23(2):309–322. doi: 10.1007/bf01046222 8331049

49. Stivers T, Enfield NJ, Brown P, Englert C, Hayashi M, Heinemann T, Hoyamann G, Rossano F, de Ruiter JP, Yoon KE, Levinson SC. Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2009;106(26):10587–10592.

50. Bögels S, Torreira F, Listeners Use Intonational Phrase Boundaries to Project Turn Ends in Spoken Interaction. Journal of Phonetics, 2015;52:46–57.


Článok vyšiel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 12
Najčítanejšie tento týždeň
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvýšte si kvalifikáciu online z pohodlia domova

Aktuální možnosti diagnostiky a léčby litiáz
nový kurz
Autori: MUDr. Tomáš Ürge, PhD.

Všetky kurzy
Prihlásenie
Zabudnuté heslo

Zadajte e-mailovú adresu, s ktorou ste vytvárali účet. Budú Vám na ňu zasielané informácie k nastaveniu nového hesla.

Prihlásenie

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte sa

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#