Chronic stability of single-channel neurophysiological correlates of gross and fine reaching movements in the rat
Authors:
David T. Bundy aff001; David J. Guggenmos aff001; Maxwell D. Murphy aff002; Randolph J. Nudo aff001
Authors place of work:
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS, United States of America
aff001; Bioengineering Graduate Program, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, United States of America
aff002; Landon Center on Aging, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS, United States of America
aff003
Published in the journal:
PLoS ONE 14(10)
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219034
Summary
While substantial task-related neural activity has been observed during motor tasks in rodent primary motor cortex and premotor cortex, the long-term stability of these responses in healthy rats is uncertain, limiting the interpretability of longitudinal changes in the specific patterns of neural activity associated with learning or motor recovery following injury. This study examined the stability of task-related neural activity associated with execution of two distinct reaching tasks in healthy rodents. A novel automated rodent behavioral apparatus was constructed and rats were trained to perform a reaching task combining a ‘gross’ lever press and a ‘fine’ pellet retrieval. In each animal, two chronic microelectrode arrays were implanted in motor cortex spanning the caudal forelimb area (rodent primary motor cortex) and the rostral forelimb area (rodent premotor cortex). We recorded multiunit spiking and local field potential activity from 10 days to 7–10 weeks post-implantation to characterize the patterns of neural activity observed during each task component and analyzed the consistency of channel-specific task-related neural activity. Task-related changes in neural activity were observed on the majority of channels. While the task-related changes in multi-unit spiking and local field potential spectral power were consistent over several weeks, spectral power changes were more stable, despite the trade-off of decreased spatial and temporal resolution. These results show that neural activity in rodent primary and premotor cortex is associated with specific phases of reaching movements with stable patterns of task-related activity across time, establishing the relevance of the rodent for future studies designed to examine changes in task-related neural activity during recovery from focal cortical lesions.
Keywords:
Medical implants – Rodents – Rats – Neurophysiology – Action potentials – Animal performance – Primates – Microelectrodes
1. Introduction
An important challenge in neuroscience is determining how the brain controls skilled forelimb movements, a topic that has important implications for motor recovery following brain injuries as well as the development of neuroprosthetic systems. Along with non-human primates, rodents are valuable models for examining the neurophysiological basis of motor control. In particular, rodents can learn to perform a wide variety of motor tasks, including: lever press/pull movements with complex timing [1], 2D center-out joystick movements [2], single-pellet reach-to-grasp food retrievals [3, 4], and even control of brain-computer interface systems with neural activity recorded from their motor cortex [5, 6]. Because of their ability to learn complex and flexible motor behaviors, rodent species have become valuable models for studies of motor control, neural plasticity during motor learning, and neural plasticity during recovery from focal cortical injury [1, 7–11].
In humans and non-human primates, complex volitional movement is a result of the output and coordination of activity in several motor areas within the cortex. While not as extensive as the primate motor system, the wide range of motor behaviors examined in rodents is likely facilitated by the presence of multiple differentiated motor areas within the rodent motor cortex. The rodent motor cortex includes two distinct and interconnected areas in which forelimb movements can be elicited with intracortical microstimulation (ICMS) and which both have direct projections to the spinal cord: the caudal forelimb area (CFA) and the rostral forelimb area (RFA) [12, 13]. Based upon the sensory response properties and overall distribution of afferent and efferent fiber connections, it is thought that CFA is homologous with the primate primary motor cortex while RFA shows similarities to the premotor cortices and supplementary motor areas (SMA) [13, 14]. While CFA and RFA are functionally distinct, both demonstrate substantial task-related neural activity during the performance of a reaching task [7]. In part, this may be due to the dense reciprocal connectivity between the two regions [13], which depending on the relative timing of excitation in each area, allows RFA and CFA to modify the output from the other area [15].
In addition to contributing to the ability of rodents to perform complex motor behaviors, the presence of multiple distinct forelimb motor areas has important implications for studies examining neural plasticity in secondary motor regions during recovery from a cortical injury, such as stroke or traumatic brain injury. While examining task-related neural activity at different stages of motor recovery may help explain the specific neural mechanisms associated with behavioral recovery, there are several considerations that need to be addressed prior to examining the specific correlates between changes in the task-related patterns of neural activity and motor recovery. First, while single-pellet retrieval tasks involving reach-to-grasp movements are sensitive in measuring motor recovery [9, 10, 16, 17], more substantial lesion models can induce significant and persistent deficits limiting successful task performance for extended periods of time [9]. Therefore, a behavioral task with graded levels of difficulty will be required to assess the neural correlates of motor recovery across the entire time course of motor recovery. Secondly, while previous studies have shown that chronic neurophysiological recordings can be acquired and used for decoding motor parameters and controlling brain-computer interface systems [5, 6, 11], the stability of the relationship between neural activity and voluntary movements at the level of individual channels is uncertain in rodent models.
This study addresses these limitations through a novel automated complex reaching task combining a ‘gross’ lever press with a ‘fine’ single pellet retrieval within a single trial. By combining these two components into a single task, it is possible to examine motor activity in a single animal while modulating the task requirements in terms of the level of fine motor control of the distal forepaw required to successfully complete the task. Additionally, we examined the stability of task-related neural activity over 7–10 weeks. Each task component was associated with robust task-related neural activity showing that both RFA and CFA are associated with specific phases of reaching movements. Furthermore, both multi-unit activity and local-field potentials were stable over periods of several weeks showing that these features can be used in future studies examining the longitudinal changes in movement-related neural activity during recovery from a cortical injury.
2. Materials and methods
All procedures were approved by the University of Kansas Medical Center Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee in compliance with The Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Eighth Edition, The National Academies Press, 2011). To examine the neural correlates of gross and fine reaching movements, five Long-Evans rats (Rattus norvegicus) were trained to perform a novel complex reaching task utilizing a custom-designed automated behavior box while neural recordings were made from RFA and CFA.
2.1 Custom behavior box
A custom automated behavior box was developed that combined a lever press and skilled pellet retrieval into a single trial (Fig 1). A 12” x 12” behavior box was constructed from acrylic sheets. Each box had 15 mm wide vertical slits cut at 30 mm from the edge of the sides of both the front and back panels. This placement allowed rodents to reach through the slits with the forepaw positioned towards the side of the box, while restricting the ability of rodents to reach through the slits with their opposite forepaw. A lever with an operating force of 0.20 N was placed directly behind each slit in the back panel with the paddle of the lever centered in the middle of the slit at a height of 28 mm, 23 mm behind the inside edge of the box. The position of the lever could be adjusted, allowing the lever to pass through the slit for temporary placement inside of the box to aid in the initial shaping of behavior. A shelf was placed spanning the entire width of the front of the box at a height of 30 mm with a gap between the outer edge of the box and the front edge of the shelf in front of the vertical slits. A depression was made on the shelf 24 mm outside of the inside edge of the box and aligned to the inside edge of the vertical slit to ensure the consistent position of the food pellet in each trial. An acrylic door was placed in front of each slit in the front panel and was controlled by a linear actuator (Actuonix, Victoria, BC), controlling access to the shelf at the front of the box. Two infrared beam break sensors provided feedback regarding the position of the forepaw to aid in segmenting rodent behavior and switching between task periods. One infrared beam was placed across the front of the box and a second infrared beam was aligned vertically through the pellet location. Levers at the back corners of the box were coupled with the door at the diagonally opposite corner, forcing rats to use the same forepaw for each task component. By aligning the height and distance of the lever at the back of the box with the pellet tray at the front of the box, both the lever press and pellet retrieval required similar ballistic reaching movements with differing requirements for fine movements of the distal forepaw and sensorimotor integration. Task transitions were controlled by an Arduino microcontroller (Arduino Uno, Arduino, Ivrea, Italy) run by a custom-made MATLAB (Version R2017a, MathWorks, Natick, MA) executable function. A webcam was attached to the side wall, perpendicular to the front of the box, and aligned to image across the length of the shelf. Video from the webcam was acquired by the MATLAB executable code at 25 fps to capture reaches to the pellet tray and to control pellet dispensing. The webcam could be positioned at either side of the box to enable testing rats with either a right or left forepaw preference. Two pellet dispensers (Med Associates, Inc., Fairfax, VT) were used to dispense pellets to either side of the pellet shelf. Finally, an LED light was used to allow us to synchronize neural activity, behavioral performance, and videos recorded from additional external video camera.
2.2 Task structure
Prior to the beginning of each trial, both doors were closed, and a pellet was dispensed to the side of the pellet shelf corresponding to the rat’s preferred forelimb for pellet retrieval. Initially, rats were required to reach through the back of the box and depress the lever on the side of the box corresponding to their preferred forelimb. Upon depressing the lever, the door at the slit diagonally opposite the lever was opened, providing access to the previously dispensed food pellet on the shelf. After detecting a completed reach through the front of the box, as indicated by the infrared beam being blocked by the rat’s forepaw and then subsequently unblocked upon completion of the reaching movement, the door was closed to limit secondary reaching attempts. If an attempt to retrieve the pellet was not made within 20 seconds, the trial was aborted.
2.3 Behavioral training
Each rat went through a procedure to gradually shape their behavior to complete the full task. Initially, rats were placed in a non-automated box with a single slit cut in the center of the front panel. Rats were allowed free access to food pellets through this opening to learn to retrieve food pellets and determine forepaw preference. Pellets were placed in the center of the opening on a shelf placed across the front of the box at a height of 30 mm. Once rats began consistently retrieving pellets, their preferred forelimb was determined as the paw that was used on the majority of retrievals. Next, rats were introduced to the automated behavior box and allowed to retrieve pellets through the opening in the front of the box corresponding to their preferred forepaw. The door was closed after each retrieval attempt to familiarize rats with the door mechanism. The shelf in the automated behavior box had a gap between the edge of the shelf and the outer edge of the box which reinforced the requirement that the rats fully grasp each pellet, and not retrieve pellets by simply dragging them into the box. After successfully retrieving at least 50% of pellets, the lever press was incorporated into the training. Initially the front edge of the lever paddle was positioned inside the box and rats were cued to interact with the lever via auditory cues and food placed near the lever. A food pellet was dropped into the box by the secondary pellet dispenser each time the lever was depressed. This immediate reward served to reinforce the lever press. After beginning to depress the lever, the lever was gradually moved to its position for the full task, 23 mm outside the inside wall of the box. Next, we paired the lever press to the pellet retrieval. After each lever press, rats were cued to cross to the opposite corner of the box using auditory cues (tapping at the front corner of the box). After beginning to combine the lever press and pellet retrieval, the secondary reward received for the initial lever press was gradually extinguished to avoid any chewing artifact during the trial period. Finally, rats were trained until they successfully retrieved at least 50% of pellets in three consecutive training sessions with 25 trials per session. While total training time was variable, most rats completed each training step in 1–2 weeks and therefore could learn to adequately perform the entire task in 5–10 weeks with 30-minute long training sessions 3–5 days per week (mean ± SE = 33.6 ± 2.75 sessions). Following training, task performance was maintained with 2–3 training sessions per week until surgical procedures could be completed.
2.4 Surgical procedures
Once rats achieved a threshold criterion of 50% successful retrievals on the full task in three consecutive sessions, we implanted chronic microwire electrode arrays into RFA and CFA. At the time of surgery, rats were 23–35 weeks old. Rats were initially anesthetized with isofluorane followed by injections of ketamine (80-100mg/kg) intraperitoneally and xylazine (5–10 mg/kg) intramuscularly. Rats were also given a preoperative dose of penicillin (45,000 IU subcutaneously) to limit the risk of infection. For the duration of the procedure, anesthetic state was confirmed by checking for the presence of a pinch reflex and corneal reflex, and a surgical level of anesthesia was maintained with supplemental injections of ketamine intramuscularly as necessary (10–20 mg/hr). Rats were placed in a stereotaxic frame and an incision was made along the midline of the scalp and the temporalis muscle was resected. A laminectomy was performed to reduce cortical swelling during the procedure. A craniectomy was then made over the sensorimotor cortex of the hemisphere contralateral to the preferred forelimb and the dura was retracted. Two 16-channel tungsten alloy micro-wire arrays with electrode diameters of 50 μm (Tucker-Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL) were implanted in each rat. Arrays were organized in a 2x8 grid with 250 μm spacing between electrodes and 500 μm spacing between rows. A silver wire from the probe was attached to a 00–80 stainless steel skull screw to act as a ground. The first electrode array was implanted into RFA with the second electrode array targeted to CFA as allowed by the size and orientation of the craniectomy and the rodent-specific pattern of vasculature within the craniectomy window. In three rats, the location of CFA and RFA were confirmed using ICMS mapping procedures as described previously [9]. In the remaining two rats, stereotaxic coordinates were used for RFA (3.5 mm anterior to bregma, 2.5 mm lateral to bregma) and CFA (0.5 mm anterior to bregma, 3.5 mm lateral to bregma) [9, 10, 18]. Each electrode array was implanted to a depth of approximately 1500 μm using a motorized micropositioner (Narishige International USA, Inc., Amityville, NY). An example ICMS map used to localize the implant locations is shown in Fig 2B. Electrode locations for all rats relative to skull landmarks are plotted in Fig 2C. The electrode locations in RFA overlap considerably across rats with some variability in the location of the CFA electrode arrays due to the limitations placed by the size of the craniectomy and vasculature pattern. After inserting the electrodes, the cortex was covered with a silicone elastomer (Kwik-Cast, World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL), a head cap was constructed from dental acrylic in situ, and the scalp was sutured around the head cap. Following the surgery, rats were given penicillin (45,000 IU subcutaneously) to limit the risk of infection. Four doses of buprenorphine (0.05–0.1 mg/kg subcutaneously) and acetaminophen (80–100 mg/kg orally) were given over the next 48 hours as analgesics. All rats were allowed to recover for 10–20 days prior to beginning neurophysiological recordings.
2.5 Neurophysiological recordings
Following the microelectrode implantation surgery, rats were given 10–20 days to recover from surgery. After the recovery period, we collected neurophysiological recordings while rats performed the reaching task in the automated behavior box. Rats performed the task 2–3 times per week for up to 10 weeks post-implant (mean ± SE = 14.6 ± 0.68 sessions). On recording days, rats were briefly anesthetized with isofluorane to ease connection of a recording headstage to each microelectrode array. The headstages, which performed amplification and digitization via an on-board amplifier chip (RHD 2132, Intan Technologies, Los Angeles, CA), were connected through a slip ring commutator (MC573, MOFLON TECHNOLOGY, Shenzhen, China) to an interface board (RHD2000, Intan Technologies, Los Angeles, CA) connected via USB to a PC computer. Neural activity was recorded at 20 kHz while rodents were in the automated behavior box performing the reaching tasks. For each session, signals were recorded while rats performed 45 trials or until 45 minutes had elapsed. Occasionally, recording sessions were truncated due to the headstages becoming unplugged prior to session completion.
2.6 Behavioral scoring
During training sessions, time stamps of each lever press and infrared beam break were recorded by the recording amplifier as digital inputs. Videos of the reach-to-pellet were captured by a webcam at 25 fps and were synchronized to the neural recordings by the MATLAB interface. Additional video recordings were also made using an external digital video camera (Sony HDR-SR11, Sony Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) at 30 fps. The external camera was either positioned in front of the box to capture a higher resolution view of the reach to the food pellet, behind the lever to capture the lever press, or above the box to capture the rat’s movement throughout the box. This orientation alternated on each recording day. External videos were synchronized by capturing an LED light in the field of view that was controlled by the MATLAB interface and simultaneously recorded by the recording amplifier. Behavioral time points were co-registered by visually scoring the video recordings. As shown in Fig 1C, times for the reach to the lever, the lever press, the reach to the food pellet, and the grasp of the food pellet were identified. The times for the reach to the lever and reach to the food pellet were both defined as the frame in which the first forward movement of the paw to begin a reach was observed; the lever press was identified as the time that the lever circuit was electrically closed; and the grasp was defined as the first video frame where flexion of the digits at the end of the reach was observed. Successful trials were defined as trials in which the rat successfully retrieved the pellet on the first reach attempt to avoid potential confounding of neural activity related to an unsuccessful initial retrieval attempt followed by a successful second attempt. The reach to the food pellet and grasp were scored in each session using the webcam videos and confirmed using the external camera videos when available. While lever press time stamps could be identified in each recording, the reach to the lever was only identifiable in the subset of sessions with the external camera placed at the back of the box. The behavior box used in the current study only allowed for syncing of a single external video camera with task performance and neural activity, however, newer versions of the automated reaching box have addressed this limitation allowing future studies to use multiple simultaneous video recordings of each task component to align the behavior to neural activity.
2.7 Data analysis
Initially, raw local field potentials from all recordings were bandpass filtered from 2Hz-200Hz and visually screened to identify noisy or broken channels to exclude from all further analyses. Data was then processed to examine both multi-unit firing activity and local field potential (LFP) spectral power changes as described below.
2.7.1 Multi-unit firing
Multi-unit spiking activity was determined using a threshold crossing algorithm. We initially compared threshold crossings with a method that calculated multi-unit activity as the combination of single-unit activity detected using an automatic detection and superparamagnetic clustering algorithm [19] with manual spike sorting used to eliminate non-physiological clusters. As task-related neural activity was similar for both the single-unit detection as well as the threshold crossing algorithm, threshold crossings were used to calculate multi-unit activity throughout the study. Specifically, raw signals were re-referenced to the common average of the non-noisy channels separately for each microelectrode array. Next, signals were band pass filtered from 300-3000Hz using an 8th order elliptic filter. Finally, spikes were detected using a threshold set at -4.5 x the RMS voltage on a given electrode for a given recording session. This threshold was chosen as it has been used to produce good behavioral decoding from multi-unit activity in studies using chronic electrode arrays implanted in non-human primates [20]. Fig 3A shows an example of bandpass filtered data from an exemplar recording session along with the channel-specific multi-unit detection threshold. Multi-unit firing rates were estimated by counting the number of spikes detected in a 100 ms window that was shifted by 50 ms between windows. To examine changes relative to baseline, 1000 one-second long periods, excluding all trial periods, were chosen at random. The firing rate during these random time periods was estimated by counting the number of spikes within 100 ms windows shifted by 50 ms between windows, and the mean random firing rate was calculated by taking the mean across all windows from each random period. Finally, the firing rate during the recording was normalized by dividing the estimated firing rate by the mean random firing rate and log-transforming. The log-transform was used to normalize the firing rate with increases relative to baseline indicated by positive values and decreases relative to baseline indicated by negative values.
2.7.2 LFP spectral power estimation
Task-related changes in the LFP spectral power were also examined. Raw signals were low-pass filtered at 400 Hz and decimated to a sampling rate of 1000Hz. All harmonics of the 60Hz power line noise below the Nyquist frequency were removed using a series of 8th-order Chebyshev notch filters. Signals were then re-referenced to the common average of all channels with neurophysiological signals separately for each microelectrode array. Examples of the raw LFP signals are shown in Fig 3B. The maximum entropy method, an autoregressive method of spectral estimation, was used to estimate spectral power [21]. A model order of 50 was selected and spectral power was estimated in 2 Hz frequency bins with bin centers ranging from 2 Hz to 200 Hz. Spectral power was calculated in 100 ms windows with shifts of 50 ms between windows to examine temporal changes. As with the multi-unit firing rate, 1000 random one-second long time periods, excluding all trial periods, were chosen at random, and the spectral power was estimated in each window. Spectral power estimates were normalized by log-transformation and were then z-scored by subtracting the mean and dividing by the standard deviation of the frequency-specific spectral power estimated from the random time windows. Positive values indicated increases in spectral power at a given frequency relative to its baseline, and negative values indicated decreases in spectral power relative to baseline. Because the high-gamma band (70-105Hz) of the LFP signal has been found to represent localized activity that is strongly correlated with asynchronous spiking [22], the average high-gamma band power was found by averaging the z-scored spectral power for all frequency bins with centers between 70Hz and 105Hz.
2.8 Single-day characterization of task-related neural activity
Initially, the differences in the patterns of task-related neural activity during the two components of the task (lever press and pellet retrieval) were characterized using a single exemplar session. For the initial comparison of task-related neural activity, recordings were excluded for each rat until at least one session in which the rat performed 36 trials (80% of the daily goal) was acquired. To allow for examination of both components of the task, the next recording session with the external camera at the back of the box in which each rat performed at least 36 trials (80% of the daily goal) was used for the single-day characterization of task-related neural activity.
For each trial within the exemplar session, several behavioral time points were identified for further analysis. Sessions with the external camera behind the box allowed us to use the external camera recordings to identify the onset of reaching movements towards the lever and the lever timing to identify the downward movement used to press the lever, while the webcam at the front of the box was used to identify the onset of reaching movements to the food pellet and the onset of grasping movements. After identifying these time points for each trial, the task-related multi-unit firing rates and spectral power were examined as described below.
For each task event considered, a period from 1 s before to 1 s after the event was examined, matching the time of peak activations observed in previous studies of rodent reaching tasks [7]. For the lever press, task-related neural activity recorded from each channel was aligned either to the reach onset or lever press based upon whether the greatest absolute trial-averaged depth-of-modulation was found by aligning neural activity to the onset of the reach towards the lever or to the lever press itself. Because the neural activity was normalized and log-transformed, signals were normally distributed with increases in neural activity relative to baseline indicated by positive values and decreases in neural activity indicated by negative values. Therefore, the depth-of-modulation was defined as the absolute value of the task-aligned neural activity. Similarly, for the pellet retrieval, each channel was aligned to the onset of the reach or onset of the grasp based upon whether the greatest absolute trial-averaged depth-of modulation was found by aligning neural activity to either the onset of the reach towards the pellet or the onset of the grasping movement. For the lever press, each channel was then classified as statistically significantly modulated and reach-related, statistically significantly modulated and press-related, or not significantly modulated. Similarly, for the pellet retrieval, each channel was also classified as significantly modulated and reach-related, significantly modulated and grasp-related, or not modulated. The statistical significance of each channel and task component’s classification was calculated to determine if the trial-averaged peak task-related neural activity was significantly different from chance using an independent samples t-test comparing the distribution of task-related neural activity across trials at the time of the peak absolute depth-of-modulation to a randomly selected distribution of neural activity. The random distribution was derived by collecting the neural activity from 1000 random time points collected from outside of any trial period. A significance level of p<0.05 was used to define significance with Bonferoni correction for the total number of comparisons tested across channels and task periods. Each channel with threshold crossings identified was classified as described above.
Each channel was separately classified based upon whether there were statistically significant increases or decreases of the high-gamma band spectral power during either the lever press or the pellet retrieval. As with the multi-unit firing, the high-gamma band was also used to give each channel two classifications: first, each channel was classified as statistically significantly modulated and reach-related, statistically significantly modulated and press-related, or not significantly modulated for the lever press, and second, each channel was classified as statistically significantly modulated and reach-related, statistically significantly modulated and grasp-related, or not significantly modulated for the pellet retrieval.
2.9 Chronic stability of task-related neural activity
Finally, we sought to examine the chronic stability of task-related neural activity in RFA and CFA. Because videos capturing the lever press were not captured for every recording day due to rotating the position of the external video camera, the analysis of the stability of task-related neural activity during the pellet retrieval component of the task was computed on each session, while the stability of the task-related neural activity during the lever press was computed only on sessions with the external video camera placed at the back of the behavior box. For each channel with a statistically significant task-related change in multi-unit firing rate in the exemplar recording session described above, trials from each recording day were aligned to behavioral time points based upon the initial classification from the exemplar recording day. Next, the daily average multi-unit firing rate was calculated for each channel. A global average time course of task-aligned multi-unit firing rate was calculated by averaging the firing rate from all trials across all recording days. Finally, for each channel and recording day, the correlation coefficient (Pearson’s r) was calculated between the global and daily averaged firing rate. The consistency of task-related high-gamma band power changes was assessed using the same procedure for all channels with statistically significant task-related changes in high-gamma band spectral power in the exemplar session. We then compared the inter-day consistency of single-channel task-related multi-unit firing rate changes with the inter-day consistency of task-related high-gamma band power changes using a Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing the distribution of correlation values between the two signal types across all rats, channels, and days for both the lever press and pellet retrieval tasks. The difference in the inter-day consistency of neural activity in RFA and CFA was evaluating using a Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing the distribution of correlation values between the two electrode arrays for each task and signal type. Finally, the difference in the inter-day consistency of the lever press and pellet retrieval tasks was also evaluated using a Wilcoxon rank sum test comparing the distribution of correlation values between the two tasks across all rats, channels, and days for both multi-unit firing rate changes and high-gamma band power channels.
3. Results
3.1 Rat characteristics and behavioral performance
All rats learned to perform the task with successful retrievals on 60–80% of trials. Table 1 summarizes the overall behavioral performance of each rat. Each rat continued to perform the task with accuracies above 50% success rates, indicating that the microelectrode implantation did not impair the ability to perform the task (Exemplar Sessions: mean ± SE = 67.4% ± 4.0%; Overall: mean ± SE = 63.5% ± 1.4%). While these performance scores are lower than reported in previous studies of pellet retrieval in rats [9, 10, 17], the gap between the shelf and box required rats to fully lift each pellet and the door restricted repeated reaching attempts, increasing the difficulty of the task. Additionally, to better isolate task-related neural activity, successful trials were defined as trials in which the rat successfully retrieved the pellet on the first attempt. The accuracies observed were similar to other studies with similar placements of pellets on a pedestal, increasing the chances of the rat dislodging the pellet off of the shelf, or in which reaching success was scored using the first attempt [3, 23, 24]. Microelectrode implant locations relative to skull landmarks are shown in Fig 2C. While microelectrodes were implanted contralateral to the preferred forepaw, microelectrode locations in the left hemisphere were reflected across the midline so all microelectrode locations could be visualized on the right hemisphere to allow for comparison across animals. Because we prioritized placing the first array into RFA given its smaller size, the locations of the RFA arrays were highly consistent across rats. The increased variability in the location of CFA arrays is due to the geometric limitations imposed by the craniectomy orientation, patterns of vasculature, and size of the microelectrode arrays as opposed to variability in the location of CFA across rats. While the position of the CFA microelectrode arrays varied, the majority of the individual electrode wires were still within the CFA motor representation.
3.2 Task-related neural activity during ‘gross’ and ‘fine’ reaching movements
Initially, we sought to characterize the patterns of task-related neural activity observed in an exemplar recording session. The task-related multi-unit firing activity and spectral power changes from several exemplar channels are shown in Figs 4–6. Each figure shows task-related changes in firing rate and task-related changes in spectral power for a single electrode site. Exemplars were chosen because of their strong task-related changes in firing rate to illustrate some of the variety in the significant task-related modulation of multi-unit firing and high-gamma band spectral power that was observed across the larger sample of electrodes. In the first channel (Fig 4), a statistically significant increase in firing rate is observed during reaching in both task contexts (Fig 4B and 4C). Specifically, the peak multi-unit firing was significantly different from chance when aligned either to the reach to the lever or the reach towards the pellet. Additionally, increases in broadband spectral power were observed when aligned to both the reach to the lever and reach towards the pellet in Fig 4D and 4E. In a second exemplar channel (Fig 5) a grasp-related increase in firing rate is illustrated. In this channel, there was a small but statistically significant increase in firing rate around the lever press and a larger statistically significant increase in firing rate observed when aligned to the grasp (Fig 5B and 5C). While this increase began before the reach towards the pellet, there was a stronger depth-of-modulation observed when aligning trials to the grasp than when aligning trials to the reach onset. Similarly, there was a small but statistically significant increase in high-gamma band power around the lever press with a much larger and statistically significant increase in high-gamma band power around the grasp (Fig 5D and 5E). Finally, other channels showed a more complex response. For example, the channel shown in Fig 6 displayed a statistically significant decrease in firing rate with a peak near the lever press and both a statistically significant decrease in firing rate with a peak 650 ms before the reach to the pellet and a statistically significant increase in firing rate immediately before the reach towards the food pellet. Based upon the largest absolute depth of modulation, this channel was classified as exhibiting significant decreases in firing rate associated with the lever press and the reach towards the food pellet. In contrast to the decrease in firing rate observed near the lever press, significant increases in high-gamma band power were observed both when aligned to the reach towards the lever as well as when aligned to the reach towards the food pellet (Fig 6D and 6E). Within each exemplar, while the peak of the task-related neural activity often occurred after the aligned task component, significant changes in neural activity often began several hundred milliseconds earlier. Additionally, as both tasks consist of a sequence of movement components, the classification of each channel should be interpreted as indicating a relationship between the observed neural activity and a task component aligned to either the reach, lever press, or grasp as opposed to a relationship to the aligned event only. Furthermore, across exemplar channels, increases in multi-unit firing rate and high-gamma band power were generally related. For example, in the exemplars in Figs 4 and 5, increases in multi-unit firing and high-gamma band power occurred at similar time points relative to task performance with similar differences in amplitude between task components. However, the high-gamma band did not appear to exhibit task-related decreases in activity that were observed in multi-unit firing. For example, in the exemplar channel shown in Fig 6, firing rate decreased around the lever press, while a statistically significant increase in high-gamma band spectral power was observed.
The range of task-related activations observed was characterized across all channels and all animals. The topography of microelectrodes classified as related to each task component is shown in Fig 7. The proportion of channels classified as related to each task component are summarized for multi-unit firing rate changes in Table 2. Of the 116 channels with threshold crossings detected, 111 channels had statistically significant task-related changes in multi-unit firing rate. While more channels had firing-rate changes during the pellet retrieval than the lever press, more channels were classified as reach-related than were classified as lever press-related or grasp-related. The proportion of channels classified as related to each task component using high-gamma band power changes are shown in Table 3. More channels had statistically significant changes in high-gamma band power than had changes in multi-unit firing rate. This may have been due to the decreased spatial specificity of LFP signals as well as the fact that LFP power changes can be detected in the absence of spiking [25]. For lever press, using high-gamma band power changes, the classification of channels was similar to the classification using multi-unit firing. Specifically, the vast majority of channels were classified as reach-related, with a smaller number classified as lever press-related. However, the high-gamma band and multi-unit classifications differed for pellet retrieval. When classified based upon the changes in multi-unit firing there were more reach-related channels, but when using high-gamma band spectral power more channels were classified as grasp-related.
3.3 Consistency of rodent task-related neural activity
In addition to characterizing the task-related activations observed in RFA and CFA, we investigated whether task-related activations in RFA and CFA were stable over several weeks. The task-related multi-unit firing and spectral power changes for an exemplar channel are shown for several recording sessions in Fig 8. On each recording day, an increase in multi-unit firing rate was observed around the pellet grasp. Because the background firing rate differed across days, the depth-of-modulation was variable across days. In contrast, a similar broadband increase in high-gamma band power was observed for each session with a similar depth-of-modulation observed in each day shown.
The stability of task-related neural activity was quantified by calculating the correlation between the daily and overall average modulation for each channel with a significant task-related modulation in the exemplar recording session. These correlations are shown for each rat in Fig 9 and summarized by task type, signal type, and cortical region in Table 4. The task-related neural activity was largely stable for each rat, decreasing only when recording quality becomes impaired, such as in Rat 1 starting around day 60 post-implant and in Rat 2 starting around 40 days post-implant. Task-related multi-unit firing rate changes and high-gamma band power changes were stable over time in both RFA and CFA for both task components. With regards to the signal type, daily changes in high-gamma band power were more consistently correlated to the overall average than multi-unit firing rates were for the retrieval but not the lever press (Wilcoxon rank sum test, Retrieval: p = 1.07 x 10−6; Lever: p = 0.63). With regards to cortical regions, the differences observed in the stability of task-related activity within RFA and CFA were not consistent across task components. Specifically, task-related activity evaluated with either multi-unit firing or high-gamma band power was more stable in RFA than CFA for the pellet retrieval, but task-related high-gamma band activity but not multi-unit firing was more stable in CFA than RFA for the lever press (Retrieval multi-unit: p<1 x 10−19; Retrieval high-gamma band: p<1 x 10−64; Lever multi-unit: p = 0.46; Lever high-gamma: p = 3.06 x 10−9). Finally, when comparing task components, task-related activity was more consistent for the pellet retrieval than the lever press (Multi-unit firing: p = 0.003; High-gamma band power: p<1 x 10−25).
4. Discussion
This study used a novel automated behavior box to demonstrate the widespread nature of reach and grasp-related neural activity in the rat CFA and RFA which are thought to be homologous to the primary motor cortex and premotor cortex respectively. Previous studies have demonstrated task-related modulation of activity during reaching tasks including center-out movements of a lever, push/pull movements of a lever, and pellet retrieval tasks [1, 7, 11, 26–28]. Importantly, these previous studies found that neural activity could be used to decode movement parameters, such as kinematics and kinetics [11, 27]. Here we examined the neural correlates of reaching movements using a novel automated behavior box that combined a gross reaching task (lever press) and a fine reaching task (skilled pellet retrieval) in close temporal succession. While previous examples of automated rodent behavior boxes have been described, our system adds the ability to examine two similar but distinct and relatively unconstrained reaching tasks in the same animals [23, 24, 29]. As with these previous systems, the use of an automated system reduces the level of supervision necessary to train rodents to perform reaching movements and eliminates many potential biases inherent in manually administered tasks.
Interestingly, a greater number of channels were active during the skilled pellet retrieval than during the lever press, potentially indicating greater cortical involvement in planning and executing the pellet retrieval than the lever press. This increased activity during the pellet retrieval is reasonable when considering that this task component is more complex and involves increased use of the distal forelimb muscles. Therefore, the increased task-related neural activity during the pellet retrieval may represent specific or increased activation of neurons during distal movements as has been observed in premotor areas of non-human primates during tasks requiring distal forelimb movements [30, 31]. Additionally, while the lever press is indirectly associated with the food reward, the pellet retrieval is directly associated with the reward. As RFA has been shown to encode an expectation of reward [28], some of the observed increases in activity may be associated with the direct expectation of the food reward.
The combination of gross and fine reaching movements into a single behavioral task provides a new tool to examine the neural basis of reaching and grasping in rodents. Previous studies have argued for a specific role for RFA in grasping based upon the patterns of movements elicited by long-train ICMS [32]. However, lesion studies have produced conflicting results with some studies showing deficits in reach-to-grasp tasks after a lesion to CFA [10, 17] while other studies have found grasp-specific deficits isolated to lesions of RFA and not CFA when temporary lesions were induced with cortical cooling [32]. Isolating the specific differences in the roles that RFA and CFA play in controlling reaching and grasping is complicated by the fact that both regions exhibit extensive task-related neural activity during reach-to-grasp tasks [7] that are likely affected in part by the complex interactions between the two regions [15]. Our data have found that task-related activity in RFA was extensively modulated both when aligned to the onset of reaching movements as well as when aligned to the onset of grasping movements, however the observed reach-related neural activity may be due to a role of RFA in planning the combined reach-to-grasp movement as opposed to a specific role of RFA in reaching as opposed to grasping. Additionally, for the pellet retrieval, task-related activity in RFA was more stable than task-related activity in CFA, suggesting a potential more preferential and consistent relationship between neural activity in RFA and grasping movements.
While it is important to note the decreased complexity of the rodent motor system relative to humans and non-human primates, because rodents have both primary and secondary forelimb motor regions, a number of studies have utilized rodent models of stroke and traumatic brain injury to examine the role of neuroplasticity in motor recovery [9, 10, 17]. In particular, ICMS mapping studies have found that expansions of RFA are associated with recovery of function following a lesion to CFA [10]. While rodents can learn to modulate activity in the perilesional cortex [6], the specific relationship between functional changes in perilesional cortex and secondary motor regions is unknown. The combined behavioral task demonstrated here is particularly relevant for examining the changes in task-related neural activity associated with post-injury neuroplasticity. While the lever press requires only a gross reaching movement, the pellet retrieval has the additional requirements of fine control of the distal forepaw and increased integration of sensory and motor information to successfully grasp and retrieve the pellet. Furthermore, the lever press task is more likely to be successfully completed in the early period of motor recovery which will allow for the assessment of the contribution of neuroplasticity to changes in task-related neural responses during the subacute period when the level of motor recovery is insufficient to allow for successful performance on the single-pellet retrieval task [9, 18].
Importantly, despite changes in neural activity due to signal degradation as well as natural changes in the animal’s behavior (e.g. daily differences in alertness or satiety), the task-related changes in neural activity were stable in individual channels over the 7–10 week period examined. While both task-related multi-unit firing and high-gamma band power changes persist over this period, the LFP high-gamma band showed more consistency across days for the pellet retrieval task. This increased stability is likely due to the difficulty in detecting the same individual neurons across days and the increased susceptibility of multi-unit firing rates to noise in the recordings. Previous studies examining chronic recordings in humans and non-human primates have observed that less than 40% of single-units were stable through a period of 15 days [33]. Additionally, changes in firing rate and spike amplitude are even seen within single recording days [34]. Despite the differences in single-unit stability, previous studies have found that modulation of neural activity during voluntary movements is largely stable in non-human primates. Within a single recording session, while there are fluctuations in the tuning of individual neurons [35], the relationship between neural activity and movements has largely been found to be stable [36, 37]. Furthermore, LFPs and, to a lesser extent, multi-unit spiking have also been shown to be stable over periods of a few days to a few years in non-human primates performing a brain-computer interface task [38]. This work extends these findings by showing that movement-related neural activity is also stable over several weeks in rodents even though rodent reaching tasks are generally less constrained than the behavioral tasks performed by non-human primates. The finding that reaching-related neural activity in RFA and CFA is stable over several weeks has particularly important implications for examining the role of neuroplasticity in recovery from a focal cortical lesion. Specifically, while multi-unit firing may be used to compare the proportion of channels with firing rate changes aligned to specific components of the task with greater temporal precision, LFP signals may be more valuable for examining changes in the relative strength of activations across days in single channels.
5. Conclusions
Collectively, we have demonstrated a novel complex rodent reaching task incorporating a gross lever press with a fine pellet retrieval into a single trial. Importantly, there were widespread task-related changes in neural activity during both task periods from microelectrodes in RFA and CFA, demonstrating a significant relationship between cortical activity and both reaching tasks. Furthermore, this cortical involvement is maintained over months with stable task-related changes in neural activity observed at the level of single channels. These results serve to further characterize the normal role of rodent primary and secondary motor areas in planning and executing forelimb movements and further establish the rat as a model species for future studies examining the changes in the specific relationship between neural activity and forelimb movements associated with neuroplasticity following manipulation to the rodent sensorimotor system.
Zdroje
1. Igarashi J, Isomura Y, Arai K, Harukuni R, Fukai T. A theta-gamma oscillation code for neuronal coordination during motor behavior. J Neurosci. 2013;33(47):18515–30. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2126-13.2013 24259574.
2. Slutzky MW, Jordan LR, Bauman MJ, Miller LE. A new rodent behavioral paradigm for studying forelimb movement. J Neurosci Methods. 2010;192(2):228–32. Epub 2010/08/10. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.07.040 20691727; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2943042.
3. Hermer-Vazquez L, Hermer-Vazquez R, Chapin JK. The reach-to-grasp-food task for rats: a rare case of modularity in animal behavior? Behav Brain Res. 2007;177(2):322–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.11.029 17207541; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1885543.
4. Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM. The structure of skilled forelimb reaching in the rat: a proximally driven movement with a single distal rotatory component. Behav Brain Res. 1990;41(1):49–59. Epub 1990/12/07. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90053-h 2073355.
5. Chapin JK, Moxon KA, Markowitz RS, Nicolelis MA. Real-time control of a robot arm using simultaneously recorded neurons in the motor cortex. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(7):664–70. doi: 10.1038/10223 10404201.
6. Gulati T, Won SJ, Ramanathan DS, Wong CC, Bodepudi A, Swanson RA, et al. Robust neuroprosthetic control from the stroke perilesional cortex. J Neurosci. 2015;35(22):8653–61. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5007-14.2015 26041930.
7. Hyland B. Neural activity related to reaching and grasping in rostral and caudal regions of rat motor cortex. Behav Brain Res. 1998;94(2):255–69. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00157-5 9722277.
8. Laubach M, Wessberg J, Nicolelis MA. Cortical ensemble activity increasingly predicts behaviour outcomes during learning of a motor task. Nature. 2000;405(6786):567–71. doi: 10.1038/35014604 10850715.
9. Nishibe M, Barbay S, Guggenmos D, Nudo RJ. Reorganization of motor cortex after controlled cortical impact in rats and implications for functional recovery. J Neurotrauma. 2010;27(12):2221–32. doi: 10.1089/neu.2010.1456 20873958; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2996815.
10. Nishibe M, Urban ET, 3rd, Barbay S, Nudo RJ. Rehabilitative training promotes rapid motor recovery but delayed motor map reorganization in a rat cortical ischemic infarct model. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2015;29(5):472–82. doi: 10.1177/1545968314543499 25055836; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4303553.
11. Slutzky MW, Jordan LR, Lindberg EW, Lindsay KE, Miller LE. Decoding the rat forelimb movement direction from epidural and intracortical field potentials. J Neural Eng. 2011;8(3):036013. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/3/036013 21508491; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3124348.
12. Neafsey EJ, Sievert C. A second forelimb motor area exists in rat frontal cortex. Brain Res. 1982;232(1):151–6. Epub 1982/01/28. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90617-5 7055691.
13. Rouiller EM, Moret V, Liang F. Comparison of the connectional properties of the two forelimb areas of the rat sensorimotor cortex: support for the presence of a premotor or supplementary motor cortical area. Somatosens Mot Res. 1993;10(3):269–89. Epub 1993/01/01. 8237215.
14. Sievert CF, Neafsey EJ. A chronic unit study of the sensory properties of neurons in the forelimb areas of rat sensorimotor cortex. Brain Res. 1986;381(1):15–23. Epub 1986/08/27. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90684-0 3530375.
15. Deffeyes JE, Touvykine B, Quessy S, Dancause N. Interactions between rostral and caudal cortical motor areas in the rat. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113(10):3893–904. doi: 10.1152/jn.00760.2014 25855697; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4480625.
16. Whishaw IQ, Alaverdashvili M, Kolb B. The problem of relating plasticity and skilled reaching after motor cortex stroke in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2008;192(1):124–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.12.026 18282620.
17. Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Gorny BP, Pellis VC. The impairments in reaching and the movements of compensation in rats with motor cortex lesions: an endpoint, videorecording, and movement notation analysis. Behav Brain Res. 1991;42(1):77–91. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80042-7 2029348.
18. Guggenmos DJ, Azin M, Barbay S, Mahnken JD, Dunham C, Mohseni P, et al. Restoration of function after brain damage using a neural prosthesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(52):21177–82. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1316885110 24324155; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3876197.
19. Quiroga RQ, Nadasdy Z, Ben-Shaul Y. Unsupervised spike detection and sorting with wavelets and superparamagnetic clustering. Neural Comput. 2004;16(8):1661–87. Epub 2004/07/02. doi: 10.1162/089976604774201631 15228749.
20. Chestek CA, Gilja V, Nuyujukian P, Foster JD, Fan JM, Kaufman MT, et al. Long-term stability of neural prosthetic control signals from silicon cortical arrays in rhesus macaque motor cortex. J Neural Eng. 2011;8(4):045005. Epub 2011/07/22. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/4/045005 21775782; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3644617.
21. Marple SL Jr, Carey WM. Digital spectral analysis with applications. ASA; 1989.
22. Rasch MJ, Gretton A, Murayama Y, Maass W, Logothetis NK. Inferring spike trains from local field potentials. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99(3):1461–76. doi: 10.1152/jn.00919.2007 18160425.
23. Ellens DJ, Gaidica M, Toader A, Peng S, Shue S, John T, et al. An automated rat single pellet reaching system with high-speed video capture. J Neurosci Methods. 2016;271:119–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.07.009 27450925; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5003677.
24. Wong CC, Ramanathan DS, Gulati T, Won SJ, Ganguly K. An automated behavioral box to assess forelimb function in rats. J Neurosci Methods. 2015;246:30–7. Epub 2015/03/15. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.03.008 25769277; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5472046.
25. Flint RD, Lindberg EW, Jordan LR, Miller LE, Slutzky MW. Accurate decoding of reaching movements from field potentials in the absence of spikes. J Neural Eng. 2012;9(4):046006. Epub 2012/06/27. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/9/4/046006 22733013; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3429374.
26. Jensen W, Rousche PJ. Encoding of self-paced, repetitive forelimb movements in rat primary motor cortex. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2004;6:4233–6. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2004.1404180 17271238.
27. Khorasani A, Heydari Beni N, Shalchyan V, Daliri MR. Continuous Force Decoding from Local Field Potentials of the Primary Motor Cortex in Freely Moving Rats. Sci Rep. 2016;6:35238. Epub 2016/10/22. doi: 10.1038/srep35238 27767063; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5073334.
28. Saiki A, Kimura R, Samura T, Fujiwara-Tsukamoto Y, Sakai Y, Isomura Y. Different modulation of common motor information in rat primary and secondary motor cortices. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e98662. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098662 24893154; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4043846.
29. Poddar R, Kawai R, Olveczky BP. A fully automated high-throughput training system for rodents. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e83171. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083171 24349451; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3857823.
30. Kurata K, Tanji J. Premotor cortex neurons in macaques: activity before distal and proximal forelimb movements. J Neurosci. 1986;6(2):403–11. Epub 1986/02/01. 3950703.
31. Rizzolatti G, Camarda R, Fogassi L, Gentilucci M, Luppino G, Matelli M. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Exp Brain Res. 1988;71(3):491–507. Epub 1988/01/01. doi: 10.1007/bf00248742 3416965.
32. Brown AR, Teskey GC. Motor cortex is functionally organized as a set of spatially distinct representations for complex movements. J Neurosci. 2014;34(41):13574–85. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2500-14.2014 25297087.
33. Dickey AS, Suminski A, Amit Y, Hatsopoulos NG. Single-unit stability using chronically implanted multielectrode arrays. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102(2):1331–9. doi: 10.1152/jn.90920.2008 19535480; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2724357.
34. Perge JA, Homer ML, Malik WQ, Cash S, Eskandar E, Friehs G, et al. Intra-day signal instabilities affect decoding performance in an intracortical neural interface system. J Neural Eng. 2013;10(3):036004. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/10/3/036004 23574741; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3693851.
35. Rokni U, Richardson AG, Bizzi E, Seung HS. Motor learning with unstable neural representations. Neuron. 2007;54(4):653–66. Epub 2007/05/25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.04.030 17521576.
36. Chestek CA, Batista AP, Santhanam G, Yu BM, Afshar A, Cunningham JP, et al. Single-neuron stability during repeated reaching in macaque premotor cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27(40):10742–50. Epub 2007/10/05. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0959-07.2007 17913908; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6672821.
37. Stevenson IH, Cherian A, London BM, Sachs NA, Lindberg E, Reimer J, et al. Statistical assessment of the stability of neural movement representations. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(2):764–74. Epub 2011/05/27. doi: 10.1152/jn.00626.2010 21613593; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3154833.
38. Flint RD, Scheid MR, Wright ZA, Solla SA, Slutzky MW. Long-Term Stability of Motor Cortical Activity: Implications for Brain Machine Interfaces and Optimal Feedback Control. J Neurosci. 2016;36(12):3623–32. Epub 2016/03/26. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2339-15.2016 27013690; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4804017.
Článok vyšiel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 10
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
- Nejasný stín na plicích – kazuistika
- Masturbační chování žen v ČR − dotazníková studie
- Úspěšná resuscitativní thorakotomie v přednemocniční neodkladné péči
- Fixní kombinace paracetamol/kodein nabízí synergické analgetické účinky
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
- Correction: Low dose naltrexone: Effects on medication in rheumatoid and seropositive arthritis. A nationwide register-based controlled quasi-experimental before-after study
- Combining CDK4/6 inhibitors ribociclib and palbociclib with cytotoxic agents does not enhance cytotoxicity
- Experimentally validated simulation of coronary stents considering different dogboning ratios and asymmetric stent positioning
- Prevalence of pectus excavatum (PE), pectus carinatum (PC), tracheal hypoplasia, thoracic spine deformities and lateral heart displacement in thoracic radiographs of screw-tailed brachycephalic dogs