Moral grandstanding in public discourse: Status-seeking motives as a potential explanatory mechanism in predicting conflict
Autoři:
Joshua B. Grubbs aff001; Brandon Warmke aff002; Justin Tosi aff003; A. Shanti James aff001; W. Keith Campbell aff004
Působiště autorů:
Department of Psychology, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, Ohio, United States of America
aff001; Department of Philosophy, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, Ohio, United States of America
aff002; Department of Philosophy, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas, United States of America
aff003; Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia, United States of America
aff004
Vyšlo v časopise:
PLoS ONE 14(10)
Kategorie:
Research Article
prolekare.web.journal.doi_sk:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223749
Souhrn
Public discourse is often caustic and conflict-filled. This trend seems to be particularly evident when the content of such discourse is around moral issues (broadly defined) and when the discourse occurs on social media. Several explanatory mechanisms for such conflict have been explored in recent psychological and social-science literatures. The present work sought to examine a potentially novel explanatory mechanism defined in philosophical literature: Moral Grandstanding. According to philosophical accounts, Moral Grandstanding is the use of moral talk to seek social status. For the present work, we conducted six studies, using two undergraduate samples (Study 1, N = 361; Study 2, N = 356); a sample matched to U.S. norms for age, gender, race, income, Census region (Study 3, N = 1,063); a YouGov sample matched to U.S. demographic norms (Study 4, N = 2,000); and a brief, one-month longitudinal study of Mechanical Turk workers in the U.S. (Study 5, Baseline N = 499, follow-up n = 296), and a large, one-week YouGov sample matched to U.S. demographic norms (Baseline N = 2,519, follow-up n = 1,776). Across studies, we found initial support for the validity of Moral Grandstanding as a construct. Specifically, moral grandstanding motivation was associated with status-seeking personality traits, as well as greater political and moral conflict in daily life.
Klíčová slova:
Behavior – Motivation – Social media – Personality – Personality traits – Personality disorders – Vigilance – Moral philosophy
Zdroje
1. Crockett MJ. Moral outrage in the digital age. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1: 769. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0213-3 31024117
2. Brandt MJ, Reyna C, Chambers JR, Crawford JT, Wetherell G. The Ideological-Conflict Hypothesis: Intolerance Among Both Liberals and Conservatives. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23: 27–34. doi: 10.1177/0963721413510932
3. Chambers JR, Schlenker BR, Collisson B. Ideology and Prejudice: The Role of Value Conflicts. Psychol Sci. 2013;24: 140–149. doi: 10.1177/0956797612447820 23287021
4. Hanselmann M, Tanner C. Taboos and conflicts in decision making: Sacred values, decision difficulty, and emotions. Judgm Decis Mak. 2008;3: 13.
5. Sheikh H, Ginges J, Coman A, Atran S. Religion, group threat and sacred values. Judgm Decis Mak. 2012;7: 9.
6. Johnen M, Jungblut M, Ziegele M. The digital outcry: What incites participation behavior in an online firestorm? New Media Soc. 2018;20: 3140–3160. doi: 10.1177/1461444817741883
7. Lamba H, Malik MM, Pfeffer J. A Tempest in a Teacup? Analyzing firestorms on Twitter. 2015 IEEE/ACM International Conference on Advances in Social Networks Analysis and Mining (ASONAM). 2015. pp. 17–24.
8. Pfeffer J, Zorbach T, Carley KM. Understanding online firestorms: Negative word-of-mouth dynamics in social media networks. J Mark Commun. 2014;20: 117–128. doi: 10.1080/13527266.2013.797778
9. Sawaoka T, Monin B. The Paradox of Viral Outrage. Psychol Sci. 2018; 095679761878065. doi: 10.1177/0956797618780658 30091685
10. Tosi J, Warmke B. Moral Grandstanding. Philos Public Aff. 2016;44: 197–217. doi: 10.1111/papa.12075
11. Castells M. Networks of outrage and hope: Social movements in the Internet age. John Wiley & Sons; 2015.
12. Boutyline A, Willer R. The social structure of political echo chambers: Variation in ideological homophily in online networks. Polit Psychol. 2017;38: 551–569.
13. Lee JK, Choi J, Kim C, Kim Y. Social Media, Network Heterogeneity, and Opinion Polarization. J Commun. 2014;64: 702–722. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12077
14. Lönnqvist J-E, Itkonen JVA. Homogeneity of personal values and personality traits in Facebook social networks. J Res Personal. 2016;60: 24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.11.001
15. Wojcieszak M. ‘Don’t talk to me’: effects of ideologically homogeneous online groups and politically dissimilar offline ties on extremism. New Media Soc. 2010;12: 637–655. doi: 10.1177/1461444809342775
16. Clark CJ, Liu BS, Winegard BM, Ditto PH. Tribalism Is Human Nature. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2019; 096372141986228. doi: 10.1177/0963721419862289
17. Spohr D. Fake news and ideological polarization: Filter bubbles and selective exposure on social media. Bus Inf Rev. 2017;34: 150–160. doi: 10.1177/0266382117722446
18. Williams HTP, McMurray JR, Kurz T, Hugo Lambert F. Network analysis reveals open forums and echo chambers in social media discussions of climate change. Glob Environ Change. 2015;32: 126–138. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2015.03.006
19. Goldie D, Linick M, Jabbar H, Lubienski C. Using Bibliometric and Social Media Analyses to Explore the “Echo Chamber” Hypothesis. Educ Policy. 2014;28: 281–305. doi: 10.1177/0895904813515330
20. Bail CA, Argyle LP, Brown TW, Bumpus JP, Chen H, Hunzaker MBF, et al. Exposure to opposing views on social media can increase political polarization. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115: 9216–9221. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1804840115 30154168
21. Bessi A, Zollo F, Vicario MD, Puliga M, Scala A, Caldarelli G, et al. Users Polarization on Facebook and Youtube. PLOS ONE. 2016;11: e0159641. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159641 27551783
22. Blatz CW, Mercier B. False Polarization and False Moderation: Political Opponents Overestimate the Extremity of Each Other’s Ideologies but Underestimate Each Other’s Certainty. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2018;9: 521–529. doi: 10.1177/1948550617712034
23. Hwang H, Kim Y, Huh CU. Seeing is Believing: Effects of Uncivil Online Debate on Political Polarization and Expectations of Deliberation. J Broadcast Electron Media. 2014;58: 621–633. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2014.966365
24. Westfall J, Van Boven L, Chambers JR, Judd CM. Perceiving Political Polarization in the United States: Party Identity Strength and Attitude Extremity Exacerbate the Perceived Partisan Divide. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10: 145–158. doi: 10.1177/1745691615569849 25910386
25. Fox J, Moreland JJ. The dark side of social networking sites: An exploration of the relational and psychological stressors associated with Facebook use and affordances. Comput Hum Behav. 2015;45: 168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.083
26. Boxell L, Gentzkow M, Shapiro JM. Greater Internet use is not associated with faster growth in political polarization among US demographic groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2017;114: 10612–10617. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1706588114 28928150
27. Saucier DA, Webster RJ. Social Vigilantism: Measuring Individual Differences in Belief Superiority and Resistance to Persuasion. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2010;36: 19–32. doi: 10.1177/0146167209346170 19776422
28. Saucier DA, Webster RJ, Hoffman BH, Strain ML. Social vigilantism and reported use of strategies to resist persuasion. Personal Individ Differ. 2014;70: 120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.031
29. Gerbaudo P. Protest avatars as memetic signifiers: political profile pictures and the construction of collective identity on social media in the 2011 protest wave. Inf Commun Soc. 2015;18: 916–929. doi: 10.1080/1369118X.2015.1043316
30. Khan ML. Social media engagement: What motivates user participation and consumption on YouTube? Comput Hum Behav. 2017;66: 236–247. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.024
31. Lee CS, Ma L. News sharing in social media: The effect of gratifications and prior experience. Comput Hum Behav. 2012;28: 331–339. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2011.10.002
32. Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull. 1995;117: 497–529. 7777651
33. Cheng JT, Tracy JL, Foulsham T, Kingstone A, Henrich J. Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2013;104: 103–125. doi: 10.1037/a0030398 23163747
34. Cheng JT, Tracy JL, Henrich J. Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evol Hum Behav. 2010;31: 334–347. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.004
35. Anderson C, Hildreth JAD, Howland L. Is the desire for status a fundamental human motive? A review of the empirical literature. Psychol Bull. 2015;141: 574–601. doi: 10.1037/a0038781 25774679
36. Hobfoll SE. Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Rev Gen Psychol. 2002;6: 307–324. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.6.4.307
37. Huberman BA, Loch CH, ÖNçüler A. Status As a Valued Resource. Soc Psychol Q. 2004;67: 103–114. doi: 10.1177/019027250406700109
38. Magee JC, Galinsky AD. Social hierarchy: The self-reinforcing nature of power and status. Acad Manag Ann. 2008;2: 351–398. doi: 10.5465/19416520802211628
39. Cheng JT, Tracy JL, Anderson C, editors. Psychology of social status. New York: Springer; 2014.
40. Holland E, Wolf EB, Looser C, Cuddy A. Visual attention to powerful postures: People avert their gaze from nonverbal dominance displays. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;68: 60–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.05.001
41. Kalkhoff W, Thye SR, Gregory SW. Nonverbal Vocal Adaptation and Audience Perceptions of Dominance and Prestige. Soc Psychol Q. 2017;80: 342–354. doi: 10.1177/0190272517738215
42. Witkower Z, Tracy JL, Cheng JT, Henrich J. Two signals of social rank: Prestige and dominance are associated with distinct nonverbal displays. J Pers Soc Psychol. In press; In press.
43. Maner JK. Dominance and Prestige: A Tale of Two Hierarchies. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2017;26: 526–531. doi: 10.1177/0963721417714323
44. Charness G, Masclet D, Villeval MC. The Dark Side of Competition for Status. Manag Sci. 2013;60: 38–55. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2013.1747
45. Mahon JE. The Definition of Lying and Deception. In: Zalta EN, editor. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Winter 2016. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University; 2016. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/lying-definition/
46. Roh S. Examining the paracrisis online: The effects of message source, response strategies and social vigilantism on public responses. Public Relat Rev. 2017;43: 587–596. doi: 10.1016/j.pubrev.2017.03.004
47. Strauts E, Blanton H. That’s not funny: Instrument validation of the concern for political correctness scale. Personal Individ Differ. 2015;80: 32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.012
48. Toner K, Leary MR, Asher MW, Jongman-Sereno KP. Feeling Superior Is a Bipartisan Issue: Extremity (Not Direction) of Political Views Predicts Perceived Belief Superiority. Psychol Sci. 2013;24: 2454–2462. doi: 10.1177/0956797613494848 24096379
49. Raimi KT, Leary MR. Belief superiority in the environmental domain: Attitude extremity and reactions to fracking. J Environ Psychol. 2014;40: 76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.05.005
50. Miller JD, Few LR, Wilson L, Gentile B, Widiger TA, Mackillop J, et al. The Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory (FFNI): a test of the convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of FFNI scores in clinical and community samples. Psychol Assess. 2013;25: 748–758. doi: 10.1037/a0032536 23647044
51. Orlitzky M. Virtue Signaling: Oversocialized “Integrity” in a Politically Correct World. In: Monga M, Orlitzky M, editors. Integrity in Business and Management: Cases and Theory. Routledge; 2017.
52. Orlitzky M. Virtue Signaling [Internet]. 1 Jan 2018 [cited 21 Jan 2019].
53. Grubbs JB, Exline JJ. Trait entitlement: A cognitive-personality source of vulnerability to psychological distress. Psychol Bull. 2016;142: 1204–1226. doi: 10.1037/bul0000063 27504935
54. Zeigler-Hill V, Vrabel JK, McCabe GA, Cosby CA, Traeder CK, Hobbs KA, et al. Narcissism and the pursuit of status. J Pers. 2019;87: 310–327. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12392 29637567
55. Zeigler-Hill V, Hobbs KA. The Darker Aspects of Motivation: Pathological Personality Traits and the Fundamental Social Motives. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2017;36: 87–107. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2017.36.2.87
56. Neel R, Kenrick DT, White AE, Neuberg SL. Individual differences in fundamental social motives. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;110: 887. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000068 26371400
57. Maples JL, Guan L, Carter NT, Miller JD. A test of the International Personality Item Pool representation of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory and development of a 120-item IPIP-based measure of the five-factor model. Psychol Assess. 2014;26: 1070. doi: 10.1037/pas0000004 24932643
58. Sherman ED, Miller JD, Few LR, Campbell WK, Widiger TA, Crego C, et al. Development of a Short Form of the Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory: The FFNI-SF. Psychol Assess. 2015;27: 1110–1116. doi: 10.1037/pas0000100 25774640
59. Glover N, Miller JD, Lynam DR, Crego C, Widiger TA. The Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory: A five-factor measure of narcissistic personality traits. J Pers Assess. 2012;94: 500–512. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2012.670680 22475323
60. Miller JD, Gentile B, Campbell WK. A Test of the Construct Validity of the Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory. J Pers Assess. 2013;95: 377–387. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2012.742903 23186210
61. Costello AB, Osborne JW. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2005;10: 9.
62. Sakaluk JK, Short SD. A Methodological Review of Exploratory Factor Analysis in Sexuality Research: Used Practices, Best Practices, and Data Analysis Resources. J Sex Res. 2017;54: 1–9. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2015.1137538 26886499
63. Rosseel Y. lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and more Version 0.5–12 (BETA). J Stat Softw. 2012;42: 1–36.
64. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2018. https://www.R-project.org/
65. Litman L, Robinson J, Abberbock T. TurkPrime.com: A versatile crowdsourcing data acquisition platform for the behavioral sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2017;49: 433–442. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0727-z 27071389
66. Altemeyer B. Right-wing authoritarianism. University of Manitoba press; 1981.
67. Conway LG, Houck SC, Gornick LJ, Repke MA. Finding the Loch Ness Monster: Left-Wing Authoritarianism in the United States: Left-Wing Authoritarianism in the United States. Polit Psychol. 2017; doi: 10.1111/pops.12470
68. Rivers D. Pew Research: YouGov consistently outperforms competitors on accuracy | YouGov [Internet]. 2016 [cited 15 May 2018]. https://today.yougov.com/topics/finance/articles-reports/2016/05/13/pew-research-yougov
69. Condon D. The SAPA Personality Inventory: An empirically-derived, hierarchically-organized self-report personality assessment model. 2017.
70. Funder DC, Ozer DJ. Evaluating Effect Size in Psychological Research: Sense and Nonsense. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019; 251524591984720.
71. Zeigler-Hill V, McCabe GA, Vrabel JK, Raby CM, Cronin S. The Narcissistic Pursuit of Status. In: Hermann AD, Brunell AB, Foster JD, editors. Handbook of Trait Narcissism: Key Advances, Research Methods, and Controversies. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2018. pp. 299–306. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-92171-6_32
72. Anderson C, Ames DR, Gosling SD. Punishing Hubris: The Perils of Overestimating One’s Status in a Group. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2008;34: 90–101. doi: 10.1177/0146167207307489 18162658
73. Chan D. So why ask me? Are self-report data really that bad. In: Lance CE, Vandenberg RJ, editors. Statistical and methodological myths and urban legends: Doctrine, verity and fable in the organizational and social sciences. New York: Routledge; 2009. pp. 311–338.
Článok vyšiel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 10
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
- Nejasný stín na plicích – kazuistika
- Masturbační chování žen v ČR − dotazníková studie
- Těžké menstruační krvácení může značit poruchu krevní srážlivosti. Jaký management vyšetření a léčby je v takovém případě vhodný?
- Fixní kombinace paracetamol/kodein nabízí synergické analgetické účinky
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
- Correction: Low dose naltrexone: Effects on medication in rheumatoid and seropositive arthritis. A nationwide register-based controlled quasi-experimental before-after study
- Combining CDK4/6 inhibitors ribociclib and palbociclib with cytotoxic agents does not enhance cytotoxicity
- Experimentally validated simulation of coronary stents considering different dogboning ratios and asymmetric stent positioning
- Risk factors associated with IgA vasculitis with nephritis (Henoch–Schönlein purpura nephritis) progressing to unfavorable outcomes: A meta-analysis