Learning to play badminton altered resting-state activity and functional connectivity of the cerebellar sub-regions in adults
Authors:
Mengling Shao aff001; Huiyan Lin aff002; Desheng Yin aff001; Yongjie Li aff001; Yifan Wang aff001; Junpeng Ma aff001; Jianzhong Yin aff003; Hua Jin aff001
Authors place of work:
Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Center of Cooperative Innovation for Assessment and Promotion of National Mental Health, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin, China
aff001; Institute of Applied Psychology, School of Public Administration, Guangdong University of Finance, Guangzhou, China
aff002; Department of Radiology, Tianjin First Center Hospital, Tianjin, China
aff003
Published in the journal:
PLoS ONE 14(10)
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223234
Summary
Previous studies have shown that sport experts are different from novices in functions and structures of the cerebellar sub-regions and the functional connectivity (FC) associated with the cerebellum, suggesting the role of the cerebellum on motor skill learning (MSL). However, the manipulation of individuals with different motor skills fails to exclude the effects of innate talents. In addition, individuals with higher motor skills often start with the MSL in their young ages. It is still unclear whether the effects regarding the cerebellum would be shown at one’s adult age. The present study was to directly alter individuals’ motor skills to investigate whether MSL (taking learning to play badminton as an example) in adulthood influences resting-state activity in the cerebellum. To this end, young adults without ball training experience were recruited as participants and were assigned randomly into the experimental group and the control group. Participants in the experimental group were asked to attend a badminton training course for 12 weeks, while the control group did not regularly attend any ball sports during this period. Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was recorded before and after the training. Results showed that compared to the control group, the experimental group had smaller amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) in right cerebellar hemispheric VI and left VIII after training. For the experimental group, right hemispheric VIII had a stronger FC with left hemispheric IV-V, cerebellar vermal IX, left middle cingulate gyrus and right hippocampus after training. Taken together, these findings suggested that MSL, at least learning to play badminton in adulthood, reduces resting-state activity in different sub-regions in the cerebellum but increases FC between sub-regions of the cerebellum as well as between sub-regions of the cerebellum and cerebral cortices (e.g., middle cingulate cortex and hippocampus).
Keywords:
Learning – Cognition – functional magnetic resonance imaging – Sports – Hippocampus – Brainstem – Eye movements – Cerebellum
Introduction
The human cerebellum, hidden underneath the occipital and behind the brain stem, is thought to be a heterogeneous structure consisting of the vermis and two hemispheres, and it has been anatomically divided into many sub-regions (i.e., vermis: vermal I-X, hemisphere: hemispheric I-X) [1]. Previous studies have repeatedly found that the cerebellum plays an important role in motor learning and acquisition of new skills [2–6]. Recently, the results from task-state neuroimaging and intrinsic functional connectivity (FC) studies demonstrated that different sub-regions of cerebellum have different functions in motor learning or skill learning, e.g., hemispheric IV-V and VIII associated with sensorimotor processing [7–11]; hemispheric VI involved in motor coordination [12–13]; vermal VI-IX responsible for eye movement control [10,14–16].
Researchers found that motor skill learning (MSL) influences cerebellar structures and functions by observing the corresponding structures and functions of sport experts and novices [17–22]. Regarding cerebellar structures, studies on gray matter (GM) showed that the volumes of hemispheric I, VI and VIIb were larger for judo athletes compared to novices [17]. The concentration and volumes of hemispheric VIII and VI were found to be larger for badminton athletes as compared to novices [18]. Studies on white matter (WM) showed that volumes of vermal VI-VII were larger for basketball athletes compared to novices [19–20]. For task-related cerebellar functions, the changes of blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal in the hemispheric VI were found to be greater for professional archers [21] and hemispheric crus I (a part of hemispheric VII) for high jumpers [22] compared to novices, when participants imagined expert-related skills. In terms of resting-state cerebellar functions, Di et al. observed that badminton athletes had stronger amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) in vermal VI than novices [18].
With regards to FC, three studies found that MSL altered the FC among different sub-regions of the cerebellum and between cerebellum and other cerebral cortices [18, 23–24]. A resting-state fMRI study by Di et al. found that the cerebellum exhibited less FC with the right anterior cingulate cortex for athletes compared to novices [18]. Wang et al. showed that gymnasts as compared to novices had lower inter-modular among cerebellum, cingulo-opercular and frontal-parietal networks [23]. On the contrary, Kim et al. found that professional golfers had greater FC between the left cerebellum and cerebral cortices (e.g., occipital lobe, temporal gyrus, parietal lobes and frontal lobes) than novices [24]. These findings may suggest that MSL alter the function integration between cerebellum and cerebral cortices. The inconsistent results regarding cerebellar FC may be related to different types of sport events and differential sport experiences between experimental and control groups. The sport event was badminton in Di and colleagues, gymnastics in Wang and colleagues and golf in Kim and colleagues. Regarding sport experiences, the experimental group in Di and colleagues received an average of 8.9 years of professional badminton training. In the study of Wang and colleagues, participants in the experimental group received an average of 16.1 years of professional gymnastics training. In the study of Kim and colleagues, participants in the experimental group were professional golfers and had a training experience of 6 to 12 years. Previous studies have found the FC varies nonlinearly with the time of motor skill learning [25]. So, the differences in sport event and sport experience may alter cerebellar FC.
Notably, all of the aforementioned studies adopted cross-sectional design. This design cannot exclude the influence of innate factors on the effects of MSL. For instance, compared to novices, athletes are often thought to have higher talents in sports [26–28]. The athletes may have been born with specific cerebellar structures and functions that would confound with the effects of MSL. In addition, athletes often start with their trainings in their childhood. It has been demonstrated that brain plasticity is larger for children and adolescents compared to adults [29]. Therefore, it is still unclear whether MSL in the adulthood can alter structures, functions and FC associated with the cerebellum.
The present study aimed to use a longitude design to investigate the effects of MSL (taking learning to play badminton as an example) on the cerebellum in adults. To address this issue, young adults, who have no any ball training experience, were recruited as participants. They were assigned randomly to two different groups, i.e., the experimental group and the control group. Participants in the experimental group were asked to attend to a badminton training course (i.e., learning to play badminton) for 12 weeks, whereas for the control group, participants remained in their common life. Both of the two groups were scanned by fMRI before and after the training. Moreover, cross-sectional studies have suggested that professional ball training including badminton training alters cerebellar structures and functions [18–20]. Compared to other racquet sports, such as golf, it is cheaper and easier for badminton learning. Therefore, we would like to choose badminton as the training item.
The present study was interested only in the effects of MSL on resting-state functions and FC associated with the cerebellum. We would use the ALFF analysis to examine the regional properties of the intrinsic neural activity in the cerebellar sub-regions. The voxel-wise FC analysis was then conducted to characterize functional integration. Based on the literatures [7–16], the cerebellar sub-regions associated with sensorimotor processing (i.e., hemispheric IV-V and VIII), motor coordination (i.e., hemispheric VI) and eye movement (i.e., vermal VI-IX) were defined as regions of interest (ROIs) used to perform ALFF analysis and voxel-wise FC.
Task-state fMRI studies found that continuous skill learning lead to decreased activity in cerebellum [30–32]. And, the ALFF of resting-state fMRI is thought to have the same underlying electrophysiological mechanism as the task-induced fMRI BOLD signal [33–34]. Taken together, we assumed that after training, the ALFF of cerebellar sub-regions associated with motor and cognition processing in learning to play badminton would decrease in the experimental group, but not in the control group. For the FC, previous studies have demonstrated that brain regions from the same functional network or co-activated in relevant tasks have highly synchronous in spontaneous low-frequency fluctuation [35–37]. Therefore, we predicted that after training, the FC among the brain regions responsible for motor learning or cognition processing involved in learning to play badminton may become stronger in the experimental group, but not in the control group.
Methods
Participants
Thirty-six undergraduate and postgraduate students (23–26 years, M ± SD = 24.22 ± 0.83 years, 15 males) were recruited from Tianjin Normal University in return of payments. Half of the participants were assigned to the experimental group (23–26 years, M ± SD = 24.39 ± 0.92 years, 8 males), and the other half to the control group (23–26 years, M ± SD = 24.06 ± 0.73 years, 7 males). These two groups did not differ in age (p > 0.1) and education. All participants were right-handed as determined by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [38]. The participants had normal or corrected-normal visual and reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders or brain injuries. The participants did not report any amateur or professional trainings of badminton or other relevant ball sports (e.g., tennis and basketball) before the experiment. They seldom played badminton or other relevant ball sports or watched the matches or videos regarding related sports before the pre-test (less than once a month in the last three years). During the intervention, the experimental group participated in the 12-week badminton training. The control group kept their routine life (e.g., study and entertainments). However, they cannot attend any professional or amateur trainings of badminton or other relevant ball sports, play related sports, or watch related matches or videos. Informed consent was obtained from each participant prior to each test of the experiment. This study was approved by the local ethical committee (i.e., the ethical committee of Tianjin Normal University and the ethical committee of Tianjin First Center Hospital).
Experimental procedures
Both the experimental and the control groups of participants were asked to attend two resting-state scans, once before the training and once after. The first scan was taken 17–10 days before the training, and the second scan was taken 1–5 days after the training.
The training lasted for 12 consecutive weeks, with 1 hour/time × 3 times for each week. The reason for the 12 weeks’ intervention was that previous studies have found that 12 weeks of training can trigger function changes in the concerned brain regions [39, 40]. Second, a semester includes 16–18 weeks. Considering that undergraduates are usually busy in social activities during the first few weeks of a semester and in exam during the last two weeks, a training program of 12-week in the mid of a semester is relatively easy to persuade participants to complete the whole experiment. The content of training is to learn playing badminton. Each time of training included three parts. In the first part, a teacher explained about badminton skills (e.g., serve, smash, anticipate the trajectory according to opponent’s body movements and fight the ball back to the left or the right court according to the position of opponent). In the second part, participants were asked to practice playing badminton in pairs, during which the teacher corrected the wrong movements or postures. In the last part, participants were asked to play badminton with their pairs freely. These three parts lasted for approximately 10, 30 and 20 minutes, respectively. In the last three times of the training, the teacher organized a badminton match. During the training, participants in the control group did not play with any ball games.
MRI acquisition
The participants were scanned by a Siemens 3T Trio scanner with a standard 32-channel head coil in Tianjin First Center Hospital. The functional (T2-weighted) images were acquired using an echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence. Acquisition parameters were as follows: TR = 2000ms, TE = 30ms, field of view = 220mm × 220mm, flip angle = 90°, matrix = 58 × 64, 25 slices/volume, thickness = 5mm, gap = 0.75mm, voxel size = 3.8mm × 3.4mm × 5mm. The scanning direction is from top to bottom. The scanning mode is interval scan, starting from the odd layer. The resting scan lasted 8min 26s for each participant. The structural (T1-weighted) images were acquired using gradient echo sequence. Acquisition parameters were as follows: TR = 1900ms, TE = 2.52ms, field of view = 240mm × 256mm, flip angle = 9°, matrix = 240 × 256, 176 slices, thickness = 1mm, gap = 0.5mm, voxel size = 1mm × 1mm × 1mm. The scanning direction is from right to left. The scanning mode is sequential scan. The T1 imaging scan lasted 4min 10s for each participant. In the scanner, participants were asked to keep awake with their eyes closed, without any motions or thinking.
Resting-state fMRI data preprocessing
The preprocessing of resting-state fMRI data were performed using a toolbox for data processing and analysis of brain imaging, DPABI V2.1 (Copyright(c) 2014; GNU GENERAL PUBLIC LICENSE; http://rfmri.org/dpabi) [41], implemented in Matlab R2012a (MathWorks, http://www.mathworks.com/). For each participant, raw data were converted into NIFTI format, and the first ten-time points of resting-state fMRI data were discarded, leaving 240 time-points for further analysis. The further preprocessing included slice timing, head-motion correction (the data of those whose head motion > 1.5 mm in any plane and rotation > 1.5° in any direction were discarded), nuisance covariates regression (including polynomial trend, Fristion24, WM, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF)), spatial normalization by information from unified segmentation (For each participant, structural image was first coregistered to the mean functional image. Then, the transformed structural image was segmented into GM, WM and CSF by using a unified segmentation algorithm. Next, functional images were normalized to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using the normalization parameter estimated during unified segmentation.), spatial smoothing (full width at half maximum (FWHM) = 8 mm × 8 mm × 8 mm Gaussian kernel), and a band-pass filter within 0.01–0.08 Hz.
ALFF analyses
The filtered data were used to perform ALFF analysis in DPABI V2.1. An ALFF map was calculated for each participant and a normalized ALFF map (mALFF) was also obtained for subsequent statistical analysis. According to the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) template [42], bilateral hemispheric IV-V, VIII, VI and vermal VI, VII, VIII and IX were defined as ROIs (The index and names of ROIs see Table 1, the locations of ROIs see Fig 1).
At group-level analysis, a full factorial design was carried out with a focus on the interaction of test time (pre vs. post) × group (the experimental vs. the control group) within each ROI in SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) implemented in Matlab R2012a. Test time is a within-subject factor and group is a between-subject factor. The significant threshold was set voxel-level corrected pFWE < 0.05 by Family-Wise Error and cluster size > 10 voxels. For each ROI, the ALFF signals were extracted, if the interaction effect was significant. Notably, the signals were based on the ROIs from the AAL template. Then, the signals were used to perform post-hoc t-tests with IBM SPSS Statistics software (Version 22; SPSS Inc., an IBM company, Chicago, Illinois). A probability level of p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
FC analyses
The filtered data were used to perform whole-brain level FC analysis in DPABI V2.1. First, ten ROIs from AAL, which are the same as those in ALFF analyses, were selected as seeds. Subsequently, the average BOLD time courses from each ROI were extracted. The Pearson’s correlation coefficients between this average time course and the BOLD time course of every other voxel were computed. Then, the correlation coefficients were converted to Z-values (zFC) using Fisher transformation for subsequent statistical analyses.
At group-level analysis, a full factorial design was carried out with a focus on the interaction of test time (pre vs. post) × group (the experimental vs. the control group) in SPM8 implemented in Matlab R2012a. Test time is a within-subject factor and group is a between-subject factor. The significant threshold was set voxel-level corrected pFWE < 0.05 by Family-Wise Error and cluster size > 10 voxels. For the zFC between each seed region and each of the other brain regions, the signals were extracted, if the interaction effect was significant. Notably, the signals of the seed regions were based on the ROI from the AAL template and the signals of the other brain regions were based on the voxel-level whole brain. Then, the signals were used to perform post-hoc analyses with SPSS 22.0 and the significant threshold level was p < 0.05.
Results
Results of ALFF
A significant interaction between test time and group was found in the right cerebellar hemispheric VI and left cerebellar hemispheric VIII. No significant interaction was found in any other ROIs. See Table 2 and Fig 2.
Regarding the ALFF of right cerebellar hemispheric VI, further analysis showed that for the experimental group, the ALFF was slightly smaller at post-test than at pre-test (p = 0.088). For the control group, there were no significant differences between pre-test and post-test (p > 0.1). The other direction of further analysis showed that at pre-test, there were no significant differences between the two groups (p > 0.1). At post-test, the ALFF was slightly smaller for the experimental group than for the control group (p = 0.078).
For the ALFF of left cerebellar hemispheric VIII, further analysis showed that there were no significant differences between pre-test and post-test in either the experimental group or the control group (ps. > 0.1). The other direction of further analysis showed that at pre-test, there were no significant differences between the experimental group and the control group (p > 0.1). At post-test, the experimental group had smaller ALFF than the control group (p = 0.005). See Table 3 in more details.
Results of FC
A significant interaction between test time and group was found between left cerebellar hemispheric IV-V and right cerebellar hemispheric VIII, between right cerebellar hemispheric IV-V and right brainstem pons, between right cerebellar hemispheric VIII and right hippocampus, between right cerebellar hemispheric VIII and left middle cingulate gyrus, between vermal IX and right cerebellar hemispheric VIII and between vermal IX and left brainstem pons. No significant interaction was found in any other ROIs. See Table 4, Figs 3 and 4 in more details.
Further analysis showed that for the experiment group, the FCs in each of abovementioned two brain areas were larger in the post-test than in the pre-test (ps < 0.05). However, for the control group, those FCs were smaller in the post-test than in the pre-test (ps < 0.05).
The other direction of the further analysis showed that at the pre-test, the experimental group had smaller FC in in each of abovementioned two brain areas than the control group (ps < 0.05). However, at the post-test, the effect regarding FC was reserved (ps < 0.05). See Table 5 in more details.
Given that the group effect regarding the abovementioned FCs in the post-test might be due to the group effect in the pre-test as a result of significant pre-test group effect, we performed ANCOVAs on each post-test FC of each of the abovementioned two regions using pre-test FCs as a covariate. Results showed that the post-test FC was larger for the experimental group than the control group (left cerebellar hemispheric IV-V and right cerebellar hemispheric VIII: 0.30±0.04 vs. 0.07±0.04 (experimental vs. control); right cerebellar hemispheric IV-V and right brainstem pons: 0.54±0.04 vs. 0.20±0.04; right cerebellar hemispheric VIII and right hippocampus: 0.48±0.04 vs. 0.19±0.04; right cerebellar hemispheric VIII and left middle cingulate gyrus: 0.34±0.04 vs. 0.12±0.04; vermal IX and right cerebellar hemispheric VIII: 0.47±0.05 vs. 0.23±0.05; vermal IX and left brainstem pons: 0.69±0.03 vs. 0.41±0.03, ps < 0.05). Therefore, the results indicate that badminton training is effective on influencing FCs, even though the FCs were different in experimental and control groups before the training.
Discussion
The present study aimed to investigate whether motor skill learning, taking learning to play badminton as an example, in adulthood influenced resting-state activity of hemispheric IV-VI, VIII and vermal VI-IX of the cerebellum and intra-modular FC within cerebellum and inter-modular between cerebellum and cerebral cortices. The results showed decreased ALFF activity in right hemispheric VI and left hemispheric VIII of the cerebellum for the experimental group but not for the control group after training, suggesting the role of learning to play badminton on the resting-state activity of the cerebellum. In addition, the experimental but not the control group exhibited stronger FC between the several sub-regions of cerebellum and some other regions of cerebral cortices or cerebellum after training, e.g., between left cerebellar hemispheric IV-V and right cerebellar hemispheric VIII, between right cerebellar hemispheric VIII and left middle cingulate gyrus, between right cerebellar hemispheric VIII and right hippocampus, between cerebellar vermal IX and right cerebellar hemispheric VIII. These findings suggest that MSL integrates motor and cognition functions between different sub-regions of the cerebellum and between cerebellar sub-regions and cerebral cortices.
The effects of learning to play badminton on the ALFF of the cerebellum
Previous studies showed that continuous skill learning affected the function of the cerebellum [30–32]. For example, in the study by Sacco et al. [31], participants attended a tango training session every day for 5 consecutive days and underwent task-state fMRI scans before and after the training. In the scanner, participants were asked to close their eyes and imagine they were walking along two parallel lines. ALFF is thought to reflect the regional properties of the brain’s spontaneous neural activity [34]. And the ALFF of resting-state fMRI is sought to have the same underlying electrophysiological mechanism as the task-induced fMRI BOLD signal [33, 34]. Therefore, in line with those literatures, the reduced ALFF in the sub-regions of cerebellum (right hemispheric VI and left hemispheric VIII) in the experimental group (compared to the control group) observed in our study may attribute to reduced activations of these sub-regions along with the skill learning of playing badminton.
As mentioned in the introduction, hemispheric VIII is related to sensorimotor [7–10]. This region was found to be activated during certain movements [7,8]. For example, in Grodd et al.’s study [7], participants were asked to perform several movement tasks with their eyes closed. They found that flexing or extending the foot activated hemispheric IV; flexing or extending hand activated hemispheric V and VIII; flexing or extending both elbow and wrist activated hemispheric V; tapping right fingers activated hemispheric V and VIII. Stoodley et al. also found that right-handed finger-tapping activated right hemispheric VIII, whereas this region was not activated in cognitive tasks that were unrelated to certain movements (e.g., language, working memory and mental rotation) [8].
The hemispheric VI in the cerebellum has been demonstrated to be involved in motor coordination in complex movements, such as visually-guided movements [13] and sequential movements [12]. For example, in the study by Alahmadi et al. [13], bilateral cerebellar hemispheric VI was activated, when participants were asked to complete a complex grip task with their right hand according to a visual cue defining the target force. Schlerf et al. [12] asked participants to perform simple (i.e., simultaneous flexion and extension of many digits) and complex movement tasks (i.e., sequences of individual digit flexion and extension) with either their fingers or toes. The results showed that hemispheric VI in the cerebellum was activated only in the complex movement tasks rather than the simple tasks.
Compared with the above tasks, learning to play badminton in real life is more complicated and it involves various kinds of motor plan and execution and cognition processes, such as, the movements of the body and limbs, hand-eye coordination and body limb coordination. The cerebellar hemispheric VI and VIII might be activated during learning to play badminton. Therefore, learning to play badminton may repeatedly activate the aforementioned sub-regions of the cerebellum, and lead to their low activities in the resting state. The findings suggest that skill learning of playing badminton decreases the spontaneous neural activity in right hemispheric VI and left hemispheric VIII of the cerebellum.
Different from the findings in the present study, Di et al. found that badminton athletes had stronger ALFF in vermal VI than novices [18], however, the present study did not observe significant changes of ALFF in vermal VI. As mentioned above, vermal VI is related to eye movement control [10,14–16]. For athletes, the stronger ALFF in vermal VI may be due to their innate advantage of playing badminton.
The effects of learning to play badminton on cerebellar-related FC
FC reflects the temporal correlation of low frequency fluctuations in the resting-state in the different brain regions [35, 43]. Brain regions from the same functional network or co-activated in relevant tasks have highly synchronous in spontaneous low-frequency fluctuation [35–37]. Therefore, the present study showed the experimental group had stronger FC between sub-regions of the cerebellum and between sub-regions of the cerebellum and cerebral cortices after badminton training, which may be related to their enhanced temporal synchronies, due to their co-activations during learning to play badminton. As mentioned above, cerebellar hemispheric VIII and hemispheric IV-V are two sensorimotor regions of cerebellum [7–11]. These regions were activated during overt movements [7–8]. Regarding the vermal IX of the cerebellum, this region is thought to be related to oculomotor movements, such as reflexive visually guided saccades and smooth pursuit [10, 14–15]. Badminton playing relies largely on dynamic visual information. For example, players track the trajectory of a shuttle and posture change of an opponent on the opposite side of a net. These processes require coordination of eye movements. Thus, co-activation for these sub-regions of the cerebellum is necessary and inevitable during badminton playing.
On the other hand, Badminton is set to play in a court with certain length and width. During badminton playing, to strike the ball back within the court, players have to intercept the shuttle from their opponents and strike the ball back with their racquets. Playing badminton involve not only motor plan and execution but also spatial perception and mental motor extrapolation (anticipation the future location of a moving shuttle). Right hippocampus is thought to be associated with the processing of spatial information, such as space navigation [44]. FMRI studies [45–48] found that when participants were anticipating upcoming outcomes of a moving object, left or right middle cingulate gyrus was activated for athletes compared to non-athletes. It is reasonable that these cerebral areas co-activate with sub-regions of cerebellum during badminton playing, leading to their stronger FC. Our findings indicate that skill learning of playing badminton not only increased intra-modular function integration within cerebellum but also inter-modular function integration between cerebellum and cerebral cortices.
Limitations
We would like to mention limitation of our study and suggest outlines for future research. Firstly, as mentioned above, the duration of MSL may influence the effect of learning on the ALFF of the cerebellum. Future studies may extend the duration of learning to further investigate the related issue. Secondly, the present study did not exclude the influences of some factors, such as motor skills and experiences (e.g., computer games, instrument trainings and driving), physical characters (height and weight) and abilities of memory, learning, and emotional control. According to the results regarding the group effects, these factors or some of them might have led to differential FCs between experimental and control groups in the pre-test. While the ANCOVA analyses suggested the effectiveness of badminton training even though there were some pre-test differences between experimental and control groups, future studies should control for those factors and reduce the group differences in the pre-test to further investigate the related issues. Finally, it is not clear whether the results can generate in other badminton-similar sports (e.g., tennis) or even badminton-dissimilar sports (e.g., running). Future studies may include another control group, who attend the trainings of these badminton-similar and badminton-dissimilar sports, to further investigate the related issues.
Conclusion
In the present study, we observed that compared to the control group, the experimental group had reduced the ALFF in right cerebellar hemispheric VI and left cerebellar hemispheric VIII after training. In addition, the experimental group exhibited stronger FC (e.g., between cerebellar hemispheric VIII and hemispheric IV-V, vermal IX, left middle cingulate gyrus and hippocampus.) after training. These findings indicate that these brain regions may be activated or co-activated during learning to play badminton, which leads to their lower ALFF and stronger FC at resting-state. The findings may contribute to understanding the changes of cerebellar plasticity induced by MSL, at least learning to play badminton in adults.
Supporting information
S1 File [zip]
The data regarding full factorial design for ALFF.
S2 File [zip]
The data regarding full factorial design for FC.
S3 File [zip]
The data used to perform post-hoc analysis.
S4 File [zip]
The data regarding the results of post-hoc analysis.
S5 File [zip]
The data regarding the results of ANCOVA.
S6 File [zip]
The data regarding the ALFF of cerebral cortex.
S7 File [zip]
The data regarding the FC between whole cerebellum and cerebral cortex.
Zdroje
1. Schmahmann JD, Doyon J, McDonald D, Holmes C, Lavoie K, Hurwitz AS, et al. Three-dimensional MRI atlas of the human cerebellum in proportional stereotaxic space. Neuroimage. 1999; 10: 233–260. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1999.0459 10458940
2. Imamizu H, Miyauchi S, Tamada T, Sasaki Y, Takino R, PuÈtz B, et al. Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature. 2000; 403: 192–195. http://doi.org/10.1038/35003194 10646603
3. Ito M. Mechanisms of motor learning in the cerebellum. Brain research. 2000; 886: 237–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(00)03142-5 11119699
4. Miall RC, Reckess GZ, Imamizu H. The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nature neuroscience. 2001; 4: 638–644. https://doi.org/10.1038/88465 11369946
5. Doyon J, Penhune V, Ungerleider LG. Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia. 2003; 41: 252–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00158-6 12457751
6. Doyon J, Song AW, Karni A, Lalonde F, Adams MM, Ungerleider LG. Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2002; 99: 1017–1022. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.022615199 11805340
7. Grodd W, Hülsmann E, Lotze M, Wildgruber D, Erb M. Sensorimotor mapping of the human cerebellum: fMRI evidence of somatotopic organization. Human brain mapping. 2001; 13: 55–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.1025 11346886
8. Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2012; 59: 1560–1570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.065 21907811
9. Wang C, Kipping J, Bao C, Ji H, Qiu A. Cerebellar functional parcellation using sparse dictionary learning clustering. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2016; 10: 188. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00188 27199650
10. Sang L, Qin W, Liu Y, Han W, Zhang Y, Jiang T, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity of the vermal and hemispheric subregions of the cerebellum with both the cerebral cortical networks and subcortical structures. Neuroimage. 2012; 61: 1213–1225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.011 22525876
11. Habas C, Kamdar N, Nguyen D, Prater K, Beckmann CF, Menon V, et al. Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. Journal of neuroscience. 2009; 29: 8586–8594. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009 19571149
12. Schlerf JE, Verstynen TD, Ivry RB, Spencer RM. Evidence of a novel somatopic map in the human neocerebellum during complex actions. Journal of neurophysiology. 2010; 103: 3330–3336. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01117.2009 20393055
13. Alahmadi AA, Pardini M, Samson RS, Friston KJ, Toosy AT, D’angelo E, et al. Cerebellar lobules and dentate nuclei mirror cortical force‐related‐BOLD responses: Beyond all (linear) expectations. Human brain mapping. 2017; 38: 2566–2579. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23541 28240422
14. Vahedi K, Rivaud S, Amarenco P, Pierrot-Deseilligny C. Horizontal eye movement disorders after posterior vermis infarctions. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 1995; 58: 91–94.
15. Grimaldi G, Manto M. Topography of cerebellar deficits in humans. The Cerebellum. 2012; 11: 336–351. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-011-0247-4 21240580
16. Park IS, Lee NJ, Rhyu IJ. Roles of the Declive, Folium, and Tuber Cerebellar Vermian Lobules in Sportspeople. Journal of Clinical Neurology. 2018; 14: 1–7. https://doi.org/10.3988/jcn.2018.14.1.1 29141275
17. Jacini WF, Cannonieri GC, Fernandes PT, Bonilha L, Cendes F, Li LM. Can exercise shape your brain? cortical differences associated with judo practice. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2009; 12: 688–690. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2008.11.004 19147406
18. Di X, Zhu S, Jin H, Wang P, Ye Z, Zhou K, et al. Altered resting brain function and structure in professional badminton players. Brain Connectivity. 2012; 2: 225–233. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2011.0050 22840241
19. Park IS, Lee KJ, Han JW, Lee NJ, Lee WT, Park KA, et al. Experience-dependent plasticity of cerebellar vermis in basketball players. The Cerebellum. 2009; 8: 334–339. doi: 10.1007/s12311-009-0100-1 19259755
20. Park IS, Ye NL, Kwon S, Lee NJ, Rhyu IJ. White matter plasticity in the cerebellum of elite basketball athletes. Anatomy and Cell Biology. 2016; 48: 262–267. https://doi.org/10.5115/abc.2015.48.4.262 26770877
21. Kim W, Chang Y, Kim J, Seo J, Ryu K, Lee E, et al. An fMRI study of differences in brain activity among elite, expert, and novice archers at the moment of optimal aiming. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2014; 27: 173–182. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000042 25539036
22. Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Larsson A, Nyberg L. Motor representations and practice affect brain systems underlying imagery: an fMRI study of internal imagery in novices and active high jumpers. Open Neuroimaging Journal. 2008; 2: 5–13. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874440000802010005 19018312
23. Wang J, Lu M, Fan Y, Wen X, Zhang R, Wang B, et al. Exploring brain functional plasticity in world class gymnasts: a network analysis. Brain Structure and Function. 2016; 221: 3503–3519. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-1116-6 26420277
24. Kim JH, Han JK, Kim BN, Han DH. Brain networks governing the golf swing in professional golfers. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2015; 33: 1980–1987. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2015.1022570 25761601
25. Ma L, Narayana S, Robin DA, Fox PT, Xiong J. Changes occur in resting state network of motor system during 4 weeks of motor skill learning. Neuroimage. 2011; 58(1): 226–33. https://dor.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.014 21689765
26. Meylan C, Cronin J, Oliver J, Hughes M. Talent identification in soccer: The role of maturity status on physical, physiological and technical characteristics. International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching. 2010; 5: 571–592. https://doi.org/10.1260/1747-9541.5.4.571
27. Werkiani ME, Zakizadeh B, Feizabadi MS, Golsefidi FN, Rahimi M. Review of the effective talent identification factors of badminton for better teaching to success. Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2012; 31: 834–836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.12.151
28. Woods CT, Banyard HG, McKeown I, Fransen J, Robertson S. Discriminating talent identified junior Australian footballers using a fundamental gross athletic movement assessment. Journal of sports science & medicine. 2016; 15: 548. 27803635
29. Müller RA, Behen ME, Rothermel RD, Muzik O, Chakraborty PK, Chugani HT. Brain organization for language in children, adolescents, and adults with left hemisphere lesion: a PET study. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 1999; 23: 657–668. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-5846(99)00024-X 10390724
30. Haslinger B, Erhard P, Altenmüller E, Hennenlotter A, Schwaiger M, Gräfin von Einsiedel H, et al. Reduced recruitment of motor association areas during bimanual coordination in concert pianists. Human brain mapping. 2004; 22: 206–215. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20028 15195287
31. Sacco K, Cauda F, Cerliani L, Mate D, Duca S, Geminiani GC. Motor imagery of walking following training in locomotor attention. The effect of ‘the tango lesson’. Neuroimage. 2006; 32: 1441–1449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.018 16861008
32. Bernardi G, Ricciardi E, Sani L, Gaglianese A, Papasogli A, Ceccarelli R, et al. How skill expertise shapes the brain functional architecture: an fMRI study of visuo-spatial and motor processing in professional racing-car and naïve drivers. Plos One. 2013; 8: e77764. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077764 24204955
33. Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M, Trinath T, Oeltermann A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature. 2001; 412: 150. https://doi.org/10.1038/35084005 11449264
34. Zang Y, He Y, Zhu C, Cao Q, Sui M, Liang M. Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain and Development. 2007; 29: 83–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.BRAINDEV.2006.07.002 16919409
35. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar MRI. Magnetic resonance in medicine. 1995; 34: 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409 8524021
36. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, et al. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2006; 103: 13848–13853. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0601417103 16945915
37. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009; 106: 13040–13045. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905267106 19620724
38. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971; 9: 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4c 5146491
39. Chapman SB, Aslan S, Spence JS, DeFina LF, Keebler MW, Didehbani N, et al. Shorter term aerobic exercise improves brain, cognition, and cardiovascular fitness in aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2013; 5: 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2013.00075
40. Nishiguchi S, Yamada M, Tanigawa T, Sekiyama K, Kawagoe T, Suzuki M, et al. A 12-Week Physical and Cognitive Exercise Program Can Improve Cognitive Function and Neural Efficiency in Community-Dwelling Older Adults: A Randomized Controlled Trial. The American Geriatrics Society. 2015; 63(7): 1355–63. http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/jgs.13481
41. Yan CG, Wang XD, Zuo XN, Zang YF. Dpabi: data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics. 2016; 14: 339–351. doi: 10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4 27075850
42. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002; 15: 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 11771995
43. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS. Functional connectivity: the principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism Official Journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 1993; 13: 5–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/jcbfm.1993.4
44. Kumaran D, Maguire EA. The human hippocampus: cognitive maps or relational memory?. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005; 25: 7254–7259. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1103-05.2005 16079407
45. Wright MJ, Bishop DT, Jackson RC, Abernethy B. Cortical fMRI activation to opponents’ body kinematics in sport-related anticipation: expert-novice differences with normal and point-light video. Neuroscience letters. 2011; 500: 216–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.06.045 21741450
46. Abreu AM, Macaluso E, Azevedo RT, Cesari P, Urgesi C, Aglioti SM. Action anticipation beyond the action observation network: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in expert basketball players. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2012; 35: 1646–1654. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08104.x 22541026
47. Bishop DT, Wright MJ, Jackson RC, Abernethy B. Neural bases for anticipation skill in soccer: an fMRI study. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2013; 35: 98–109. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.35.1.98 23404883
48. Wimshurst ZL, Sowden PT, Wright M. Expert–novice differences in brain function of field hockey players. Neuroscience. 2016; 315: 31–44. https://dor.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.11.064 26674059
Článok vyšiel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 10
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
- Nejasný stín na plicích – kazuistika
- Masturbační chování žen v ČR − dotazníková studie
- Úspěšná resuscitativní thorakotomie v přednemocniční neodkladné péči
- Fixní kombinace paracetamol/kodein nabízí synergické analgetické účinky
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
- Correction: Low dose naltrexone: Effects on medication in rheumatoid and seropositive arthritis. A nationwide register-based controlled quasi-experimental before-after study
- Combining CDK4/6 inhibitors ribociclib and palbociclib with cytotoxic agents does not enhance cytotoxicity
- Experimentally validated simulation of coronary stents considering different dogboning ratios and asymmetric stent positioning
- Prevalence of pectus excavatum (PE), pectus carinatum (PC), tracheal hypoplasia, thoracic spine deformities and lateral heart displacement in thoracic radiographs of screw-tailed brachycephalic dogs