#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Characterising the harmonic vocal repertoire of the Indian wolf (Canis lupus pallipes)


Authors: Sougata Sadhukhan aff001;  Lauren Hennelly aff002;  Bilal Habib aff001
Authors place of work: Department of Animal Ecology and Conservation Biology, Wildlife Institute of India, Dehradun, India; aff001;  Mammalian Ecology and Conservation Unit, Veterinary Genetics Laboratory, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America aff002
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(10)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216186

Summary

Vocal communication in social animals plays a crucial role in mate choice, maintaining social structure, and foraging strategy. The Indian grey wolf, among the least studied subspecies, is a social carnivore that lives in groups called packs and has many types of vocal communication. In this study, we characterise harmonic vocalisation types of the Indian wolf using howl survey responses and opportunistic recordings from captive and nine packs (each pack contains 2–9 individuals) of free-ranging Indian wolves. Using principal component analysis, hierarchical clustering, and discriminant function analysis, we found four distinct vocalisations using 270 recorded vocalisations (Average Silhouette width Si = 0.598) which include howls and howl-barks (N = 238), whimper (N = 2), social squeak (N = 28), and whine (N = 2). Although having a smaller body size compared to other wolf subspecies, Indian wolf howls have an average mean fundamental frequency of 422 Hz (±126), which is similar to other wolf subspecies. The whimper showed the highest frequency modulation (37.296±4.601) and the highest mean fundamental frequency (1708±524 Hz) compared to other call types. Less information is available on the third vocalisation type, i.e. ‘Social squeak’ or ‘talking’ (Mean fundamental frequency = 461±83 Hz), which is highly variable (coefficient of frequency variation = 18.778±3.587). Lastly, we identified the whine, which had a mean fundamental frequency of 906Hz (±242) and is similar to the Italian wolf (979±109 Hz). Our study’s characterisation of the Indian wolf’s harmonic vocal repertoire provides a first step in understanding the function and contextual use of vocalisations in this social mammal.

Keywords:

analýza hlavních komponent – Social communication – Physiological parameters – Acoustics – Hierarchical clustering – Vocalization – Geological surveys – Wolves

Introduction

Vocalisation plays a critical role in social animals for conveying information on foraging, reproductive, and social behaviours [17]. Characterising the vocal repertoire of a species provides a base for understanding the behavioural significance of different vocalisations and studying how vocal communication varies across populations, subspecies, and taxa [810].

The wolf (Canis lupus) is a social mammal and uses a variety of vocalisations for communication. Being present throughout Eurasia and North America, the wolf is one of the most widely distributed land mammals and occupies a wide range of different habitat types [11]. While there has been much research on wolves in North America and Europe, much less has been done on the wolves of Asia. For the grey wolf, most of the mitochondrial diversity is centred in southern and central Asia, where two independent and phylogenetically basal maternal lineages–the Tibetan and Indian wolf–are found [1214]. The Tibetan and Indian wolf maternal lineages are estimated to have diverged around 700,000, and 300,000 years ago, respectively [12,15,16]. Despite its phylogenetic position as one of the oldest maternal lineages and among the smallest subspecies [12], relatively little is known about Indian wolf ecology and behaviour compared to other wolf subspecies. Studying the vocalisations of the Indian wolf can offer a greater understanding of the behavioural function of different vocal signals in Indian wolves and, more broadly, the variation in vocalisation and associated behaviour across subspecies and taxa within the Canis clade.

The best-known wolf vocalisation–the howl–is a long-range harmonic call used for territorial advertising and social cohesion [1,1719]. Wolf howl acoustic structure has been shown to vary across individuals [1,2024], groups [25], and subspecies [8,26]. Among the Canis clade, smaller species generally have howls that end in a sharp drop in frequency and a greater diversity of howl type usages [8]. Previous research has shown that Indian wolf howls generally have a higher mean fundamental frequency compared to other wolf subspecies, which may be attributed to its smaller body size [26]. Using a larger set of howls that are statistically classified by their acoustic features can provide a more robust description of the characteristics and diversity of Indian wolf howl types.

Along with the howl, wolves also communicate using seven to twelve other harmonic calls [2729]. Harmonic calls are produced by the vibration of vocal folds in the larynx, which results in a series of multiple integral frequencies of the fundamental frequency [30]. Many of these other harmonic vocalisations are short-ranged, and due to difficulties in recording these calls, remain less studied compared to the wolf howl [31]. These short-ranged calls are essential for communicating passive or aggressive behaviour among social canids [3133]. Grey wolves also use non-pitched or noisy calls, which are produced by the acoustic resonance of the vocal tract [19,3436]. Instead of a specific frequency band, noisy calls possess concentrated acoustic energy around a particular frequency range. Therefore noisy calls do not have a clear pitch or distinct frequency band in their spectrograms [30].

The whimper, whine and yelp are various harmonic calls for communicating passive and friendly behaviour among wolves [19,32], whereas noisy calls such as growl and bark indicate varying levels of aggression [19,32]. The whimper, and whine vocalisations are similar to a crying sound with the whimper having a comparatively shorter duration than whine [19,34]. The whine vocalisation is mostly used for submissive behaviour, whereas the whimper is primarily used for greeting [19]. The yelp is a short and sharp cry vocalisation that is associated with submissive behaviour involving body contacts [19,34]. To communicate different levels of aggression behaviours, wolves use noisy calls, which consist of the growl, woof, and bark. Growl is a non-harmonic sound to show dominance in any interaction, whereas the woof vocalisation is a non-harmonic sound cue used by adults for their pups [19,34,37]. The bark is a short, low pitched sound with rapid frequency modulation and is used during aggressive defence [27,37], such as defending pups or defending a food resource [4,38]. Wolves also express communication through mixed vocalisation either by ‘successive emission’ or by ‘superimposition’ of two or more sound types [29]. A recent study on the Italian wolf (Canis lupus italicus) suggests six other types of calls may combine with howls to make a complex chorus vocalisation [39].

This study investigates the acoustic structure of harmonic vocalisations of Indian wolves and classifies these harmonic vocalisations using a statistical approach. We accumulated the vocalisation data from free-ranging and captive Indian wolves, which will be the first study to evaluate different types of vocalisations of this wolf subspecies. Using multivariate analyses, we describe and classify different harmonic calls to develop a vocal repertoire of the Indian wolf.

Materials and methods

Study species

The Indian wolf (Canis lupus pallipes) is among the smallest wolf subspecies with an average body weight of 20.75 kg [40]. Indian wolves are mostly found in grasslands and the edges of dense tropical deciduous forest on the Indian subcontinent [4044]. The average home range of a pack varies from 180–250 km2 [40]. We recorded vocalisations from nine packs of free-ranging wolves and ten captive wolves from Jaipur Zoo. For captive wolves, we collected vocalisation data from 10 wolves: two adult pairs and six subadults. One adult male was recently captured from the wild near the city of Jaipur, Rajasthan, India. The rest of the Indian wolves are descendants of captive breeders at Jaipur Zoo.

Study sites

This study was conducted in the state of Maharashtra (Fig 1) and Jaipur Zoo of Jaipur, Rajasthan, India. The study site in Maharashtra was located on the central Deccan Plateau [45], which consists of the overlapping habitat of tropical dry deciduous forest, grassland, savanna (Western part) and tropical moist deciduous forest (Eastern part) [46].

Fig. 1. Map of survey sites of the free-ranging wolves.
Map of survey sites of the free-ranging wolves.
(Spatial Data Source: Political boundaries from Natural Earth, and Land Use/Land Cover data from USGS Earth Resources Observatory and Science (EROS) Center).

Data collection

Vocalisations of free-ranging wolves were recorded through acoustics survey from November 2015 to June 2016. The majority of the long-distance vocalisation recordings were collected through howling surveys to elicit howl behaviour. Opportunistically, spontaneous howls were also recorded. For other types of vocalisation data, we relied on opportunistic recordings from free-ranging wolves and captive wolves. Howl surveys were performed during early morning and evening hours using pre-recorded howls that were previously recorded from the Jaipur Zoo Indian wolves. Each howling session consisted of five trials with three minute long intervals [3]. A 50-second-long pre-recorded sequence of a solo howl was played three times using JBL Xtreme speakers (Harman Internation Industries, 2014) in order of increasing volume [3]. The session was followed by two 50-second-long chorus howls. In the case of a howling response, the session was terminated and repeated after 15 to 20 minutes [3]. Responses were recorded using Blue Yeti Pro Microphone (Blue Microphone, 2011) attached with Zoom H4N Handheld Audio Recorder (Zoom Corporation, 2009) at a sampling rate of 44.1KHz on 16-bit depth with 80 Hz noise filter. Along with howl surveys in the field, opportunistic recording sessions were conducted near wild Indian wolf den sites and rendezvous sites. In addition to howl surveys at Jaipur Zoo, vocalisations of captive wolves were recorded by installing microphones in the front of cages during closing hours (6:30 pm-7: 30 am).

Ethical approval

The study on captive wolves in zoos was done with the permission of the Director of Jaipur Zoo and the Forest Department of Rajasthan, India [Letter no- 3(04)-II/CCFWL/2013/4586-87; Dated 30th Oct 2015]. The survey of free-ranging wolves of Maharashtra was performed with the consent of the Principal Chief Conservator of Maharashtra Forest Department [Letter no- 22(8)/WL/CR-947(14–15)/1052/2015-16; Dated- 6th Aug 2015]. No animal was harmed during the study, and the standard non-invasive protocol of howling survey was maintained.

Feature extraction

We focused our analysis on harmonic vocalisations and excluded noisy calls since they do not possess a clear spectral band. Spectrograms of each vocalisation were generated through the Raven Pro 1.5 software [47] using the Discrete Fourier Transform (DFT) algorithm. The discrete Fourier function transforms the same length sequence of equally spaced sample points (N, where N is a prime number) with circular convolution being implemented on the points [48]. Hann windows were used at the rate of 1800 samples on 35.2 Hz 3dB filter. A total of 270 spectrograms were selected for further analysis based on clarity (i.e. clearer spectrogram with low noise and without external sound overlap). Web plot digitiser v3 [49] was used for digitising fundamental frequency from the spectral images. This digitised data was obtained at 0.1sec resolution. From this data, eleven acoustic variables (Table 1) were obtained based on their performance from previous studies [20,22].

Tab. 1. Acoustic variables based on fundamental frequency (f0) that were extracted for this study.
Acoustic variables based on fundamental frequency (f<sub>0</sub>) that were extracted for this study.

Statistical analysis

Principal Component Analysis (PCA)

To obtain a smaller set of variables that explain most of the dataset’s variation, we used a principal component analysis (PCA), which is an unsupervised statistical approach that extracts linearly uncorrelated variables from a suite of potentially correlated variables [50]. To simplify the interpretation of factors, we performed varimax rotation using Kaiser normalisation [51]. From our dataset of 270 vocalisations, we used eight scalar variables that are related to spectral structure (Range f, Duration, Abrupt0.025, Abrupt0.05, Abrupt0.1, Stdv, Co-fm, Co-fv) for PCA analysis through the software SPSS (v22). The first principal component (PC1) and second principal component (PC2) were used in the subsequent clustering analyses.

Cluster analysis

To classify the recorded vocalisations from the Indian wolf, we used agglomerative hierarchical clustering through the R package AGglomerative NESting (AGNES)[52]. The agglomerative hierarchical clustering algorithm measures the dissimilarity between single and groups of observations using a “bottom-up” approach, thereby constructing clusters [53]. Agglomerative hierarchical clustering was performed using Euclidean distances with PC1 and PC2 from the 270-vocalisation data using eight scalar variables. Subsequently, silhouette clustering was combined with AGNES to validate the number of clusters in our vocalisation data. Silhouette clustering measures the similarity of observation within its cluster compared to other clusters [54]. The average silhouette value (0 represents poor fit, 1 depicts the highest fit) describes the evaluation of clustering validity [54]. Average Silhouette width (Si) was calculated for 14 different solutions (2 to 15 clusters). The “solution” that provided the best fit was selected upon the maximum average silhouette value. The dendrogram was plotted using ‘Circlize Dendrogram’ in the package ‘Dendextend’ in program R [55].

Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA)

Discriminant function analysis (DFA) was performed using PC1 and PC2 as an independent variable under the program SPSS (v22) to cross-validate the obtained clusters from AGNES. Predicted clusters that were determined by the maximum silhouette value were then used as a grouping variable to evaluate within-group covariance in DFA analysis. From these clusters, we then used the box plot to show the overall pattern and distribution characteristics of different vocal clusters.

Results

Principal component analysis

Two principal components (PC1 and PC2) were generated from the eight simple scalar variables through PCA based on Kaiser-Guttman Rule (Eigenvalue >1) [56]. PC1 and PC2 together explained 70.6% variance. PC1 was based on the variances of six acoustic parameters (Abrupt0.025, Abrupt0.1, Abrupt0.05, Co-fv, Range f, Stdv) whereas PC2 is explained by the variances of five parameters (Abrupt0.1, Co-fm, Duration, Range f, Stdv) (Table 2).

Tab. 2. The loadings of PC1 and PC2.
The loadings of PC1 and PC2.
Communalities are the proportional factors by which the importance of each variable is explained.

Cluster analysis

The highest silhouette value (Si = 0.598) was obtained at the 4-group solution in the cluster analysis using PC1 and PC2 from PCA analysis (Fig 2). The average silhouette value was 0.62 for the first cluster (N = 238), 0.37 for the second cluster (N = 2), 0.38 for the third cluster (N = 28) and 0.73 for the fourth cluster (N = 2) (Fig 3). The 4 clusters were formed at 3.9 clustering scale through agglomerative hierarchical clustering (Fig 4)

Fig. 2. Average silhouette width plotted against 14 different solutions (2–15 cluster).
Average silhouette width plotted against 14 different solutions (2–15 cluster).
Average Silhouette width represents the significance level (0 represents poor fit, 1 represents best fit). We obtained maximum silhouette width in 4 cluster solutions, i.e. Si = 0.598.
Fig. 3. Silhouette plot showing the validation for the consistency among the clusters.
Silhouette plot showing the validation for the consistency among the clusters.
This plot assesses the similarity or difference of each call from its clusters. A negative value indicates the chance of a call to fall under the neighbouring cluster. The average silhouette value of 4 groups are 0.62 (N = 238), 0.37 (N = 2), 0.38 (N = 28), 0.73 (N = 2) respectively.
Fig. 4. Cluster Diagram obtained from Agglomerative hierarchical clustering using Euclidean Distance as matric function.
Cluster Diagram obtained from Agglomerative hierarchical clustering using Euclidean Distance as matric function.
Four clusters were formed at 3.9 Clustering scale. Cluster 1 (Howl), Cluster 2 (Whimper), Cluster 3 (Social Squeak) and Cluster 4 (Whine) are denoted by the colours blue, red, green and brown, respectively.

Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA)

DFA achieved 95.9% accuracy of vocal group identification using two PCA values (Table 3). Each of the four groups has a distinct group centroid. The graphical representation using two discriminant functions (DF1 and DF2) shows that vocal clusters do not overlap (Fig 5).

Fig. 5. Plot for Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA) using PCA values for 270 vocalisation data from the Indian wolf.
Plot for Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA) using PCA values for 270 vocalisation data from the Indian wolf.
Different colours represent different call type.
Tab. 3. Classification results of Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA).
Classification results of Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA).
95.9% of the vocal clusters (estimated from Agglomerative hierarchical clustering) are identified correctly.

The whisker box plot represents the variation among acoustic variables within the four identified call types (Fig 6). Call type 1 had the longest duration (5.214±2.49 Sec) whereas call type 2 showed the shortest duration among the four recognised groups (0.4±0) (N = 2). Type 2 calls also have high-frequency modulation (37.296±4.601) (variation in frequency per unit time). However, frequency variation (around the mean) is highest in type 3 calls (18.778±3.587) (Table 4).

Fig. 6. Box plot of variation among acoustic variables between different call type.
Box plot of variation among acoustic variables between different call type.
a. Variation among minimum frequency, b. Variation among maximum frequency, c. Variation among Mean frequency, d. Variation among duration of the call, e. Variation among coefficient of frequency modulation, f. variation among coefficient of frequency variation.
Tab. 4. Variation among important acoustic variables within the four identified call types.
Variation among important acoustic variables within the four identified call types.

Discussion

This study provides a quantitative assessment of the vocalisations of the Indian wolf subspecies. Our results show that there are four statistically classified groups of Indian wolf vocalisations based on ten captive individuals and nine free-ranging Indian wolf packs. Though the Four to Six solution groups showed a narrow difference in their average silhouette values based on silhouette plot analysis, the four cluster solution was found to be the most significant based on the global maxima. This characterisation of vocalisations provides a first step to evaluating the function and contextual use of different types of vocalisations in these canids.

The first, most prolonged (5.214±2.49 sec) call type in our dataset is identified as a howl (Fig 7a). The fundamental frequency of the howl ranged from 359 Hz (±116) minimum to 469 Hz (±141) maximum (N = 238). Despite having a smaller body size, the mean fundamental frequency of the Indian wolf howl (422±126 Hz) was similar to the mean fundamental frequency of other wolf subspecies reported in previous studies [26]. This contrasts previous research that described Indian wolves as having a higher mean frequency in howls [26]. Our study’s large sample size of individuals and use of a classification model to statistically discriminate vocalizations may have aided in excluding other vocal types–such as howl barks–in our analyses to robustly describe Indian wolf howls. Additionally, variation in howl acoustic structure has been suggested to be partly individual-specific, which may be due to a combination of differences in body sizes, age class, or gross anatomy [1,2024]. For example, the mean fundamental frequency of 11 Iberian wolf individuals was reported to range from 332Hz (±47) to 666Hz (±60) [21]. This high acoustic variation associated with individual wolves highlights the importance of having a large enough sample size of individual wolves to robustly describe vocal types and assess individual-specific variation within a population. To further understand the influence of body size on wolf howl acoustic structure, it would be important to identify howls using a classification-guided approach across all vocalization data of various subspecies as well as incorporating information of each howl’s associated wolf weight and individual’s identity.

Fig. 7. Spectrograms of different types of vocalisations of the Indian wolf.
Spectrograms of different types of vocalisations of the Indian wolf.
a. Howl (type 1); b. Bark-Howl (type 1 subtype); c. Whimper (type 2), d. Social Squeak (type 3); e. Whine (Type 4).

Since the howl is the most detectable vocalisation used in long-range social cohesion and territorial advertisement [8,31], our high howl sample size shouldn’t be considered as the most frequent vocalisation. Barking-howl, which was mentioned by many authors as a common type of mix vocalisation in wolves [31,35,39], falls under the same cluster along with howling (Fig 7b). From our field observations, wolves bark in defence to an immediate threat. In one such occasion, the she-wolf of a pack started barking at nearby villagers to protect her pups and did not stop until all the three pups ran away to a safer distance from the villagers.

While the howl has been extensively studied in its behavioural function and variation across subspecies [8,26], less is known about short-range communication among wolves. Our study has identified and described three short-range communication call-types found in Indian wolves. Corresponding to our results, the second call type has the highest frequency modulation (37.296±4.601) and is commonly known as a whimper (Fig 7C). The whimper is low intensity but high-pitched sound that is used for short-distance communication among pack members [28,31] (mean fundamental frequency = 1708±524 Hz). This short duration (0.4±0 sec) vocalisation is reported to be associated with submissive or friendly greeting behaviour [31,35]. Since it is not audible from more than one to two hundred yards away [35], our dataset contains only a few observations (from different packs) of this type of call (N = 2). While our study provides some initial insight into the acoustic structure of this vocalisation, further sampling will be needed to characterize the acoustic structure of the whimper robustly.

The third group of Indian wolf vocalisations can be termed as ‘social squeak’ (Fig 7C), following observations by previous studies (Mech 1981, Crisler 1959, Fentress 1967). This high-frequency variable vocalisation (18.778±3.587) in the Indian wolf is similar to ‘talking’, which was defined as ‘hovering around one pitch’ [57]. The social squeak is considered to be context-dependent, with variation within the call type being dependent on differing social interactions among individuals [58]. Otherwise, there is little known about its function in wolf packs and if it’s a common communication across different canid species and within domestic dogs. Our results suggest that the social squeak has a minimum frequency of 327 Hz (±51) to Maximum of 623 Hz (±77) for Indian wolves (N = 28).

Lastly, our fourth vocal group we identified as the whine (Fig 7E), which is characterized as a short duration vocalisation (1.2±0.707 Sec). The whine is mainly used during stressful situations, such as pack separation and/or intra-pack conflict [30]. Additionally, female wolves have also been reported to whine during the nursing of pups in the den [59]. The whine in the Indian wolf (Canis lupus pallipes) is longer than the whine reported in Italian wolf (0.13±0.10 sec), which the Indian wolf has comparatively larger body size than Indian wolf [39]. Although our data set is too small (N = 2; from two different individuals) to interpret robustly, the mean fundamental frequency of Indian wolf whine (906±242 Hz) has a similar frequency as the Italian wolf (979±109 Hz) [39].

Arising from the challenges of monitoring elusive and low-density species, acoustic methods for detection and estimating population parameters has become increasingly utilized in wildlife management [60,61]. Early wolf biologists had recognized its effectiveness for detection [62], and further statistical work on howl acoustic structure has improved its ability to monitor wolf populations [6365]. Statistically validating wolf howls from other vocalisations using an unsupervised classification technique avoids having a human biased sample of vocalisations for performing subsequent behavioural and statistical analyses, such as for identifying individuals [43]. It is important to note that howls can be context-dependent, in which individuals’ howl acoustic structure can vary according to certain behavioural contexts [24]. Since the howls were recorded from both elicited and spontaneous responses, our study’s characterization of the howl should be taken with caution, as it may comprise of multiple context-specific howls.

Further research on a larger dataset of Indian wolf vocalisations can develop a more robust classification of the vocal repertoire of this subspecies. Additionally, we defined call types in our study based on similarity to previously defined call types, such as whimper, whine, and social squeaks [35,66]. Incorporating information on the behaviour associated with these call types would aid in describing and validating the call types in our study. Therefore, statistical classification coupled with behavioural monitoring through a visual recorder is one future avenue of research, which will aid in decoding wolf behaviour in the context of its vocalisation. More broadly, the species within the Canis clade vary in their body sizes, social structure, and habitats [67]. The diversity of social complexity and vocal communication across species within Canis represents a unique system to address questions on the relationship between vocal communication and social complexity [6870]. Therefore, describing the vocal repertoires of various canid taxa provides a first step into understanding the ecological, social, and phylogenetic factors influencing the diversity of vocal communication within the genus Canis.

Supporting information

S1 File [pdf]
The variables, clusters and other details information of every calls.


Zdroje

1. Theberge JB, Falls JB. Howling as a means of communication in timber wolves. Am Zool. 1967;7: 331–338. doi: 10.1093/icb/7.2.331

2. Kingston T, Lara MC, Jones G, Akbar Z, Kunz TH, Schneider CJ. Acoustic divergence in two cryptic Hipposideros species: a role for social selection? Proc R Soc Biol Sci. The Royal Society; 2001;268: 1381–6. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1630 11429138

3. Harrington FH, Mech DL. An analysis of howling response parameters useful for wolf pack censusing. J Wildl Manage. 1982;46: 686–693. doi: 10.2307/3808560

4. Scott JP. The Evolution of Social Behavior in Dogs and Wolves. Am Zool. 1967;7: 373–381.

5. Garber P a., Estrada A, Bicca-Marques JC, Heymann EW, Strier KB. South American Primates: Comparative Perspectives in the Study of Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. 2009. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-78705-3

6. Nawroth C, Brett JM, McElligott AG. Goats display audience-dependent human-directed gazing behaviour in a problem-solving task. Biol Lett. 2016;12: 2016–2019. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0283 27381884

7. Gazzola A, Avanzinelli E, Mauri L, Scandura M, Apollonio M. Temporal changes of howling in south European wolf packs. Ital J Zool. Taylor & Francis; 2002;69: 157–161. doi: 10.1080/11250000209356454

8. Kershenbaum A, Root-Gutteridge H, Habib B, Koler-Matznick J, Mitchell B, Palacios V, et al. Disentangling canid howls across multiple species and subspecies: Structure in a complex communication channel. Behav Processes. Elsevier B.V.; 2016;124: 149–157. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.01.006 26809021

9. Tembrock G. Acoustic behaviour of mammals. In Acoustic Behavior of Animals (René Guy Busnel). Elsevier; 1963. pp. 751–786. Available: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/260796117

10. Wilkins MR, Seddon N, Safran RJ. Evolutionary divergence in acoustic signals: causes and consequences. Trends Ecol Evol. Elsevier Current Trends; 2013;28: 156–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.002 23141110

11. Mech DL, Boitani L, (IUCN SSC Wolf Specialist Group). The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2010. 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2010-4.RLTS.T3746A10049204.en Copyright:

12. Sharma DK, Maldonado JE, Jhala Y V, Fleischer RC. Ancient wolf lineages in India. Proc R Soc London B Biol Sci. The Royal Society; 2004;271: S1—S4. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0071 15101402

13. Werhahn G, Senn H, Kaden J, Joshi J, Bhattarai S, Kusi N, et al. Phylogenetic evidence for the ancient Himalayan wolf: towards a clarification of its taxonomic status based on genetic sampling from western Nepal. R Soc Open Sci. The Royal Society Publishing; 2017;4: 170186. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170186 28680672

14. Ersmark E, Klütsch CFC, Chan YL, Sinding M-HS, Fain SR, Illarionova NA, et al. From the Past to the Present: Wolf Phylogeography and Demographic History Based on the Mitochondrial Control Region. Front Ecol Evol. Frontiers; 2016;4: 134. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2016.00134

15. Aggarwal RK, Kivisild T, Ramadevi J, Singh L. Mitochondrial DNA coding region sequences support the phylogenetic distinction of two Indian wolf species. J Zool Syst Evol Res. 2007;45: 163–172. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0469.2006.00400.x

16. Shrotriya S, Lyngdoh S, Habib B. Wolves in Trans-Himalayas: 165 years of taxonomic confusion. Curr Sci. JSTOR; 2012;103: 885–887.

17. Harrington FH, Mech DL. Wolf Howling and Its Role in Territory Maintenance. Behaviour. 1978;68: 207–249. Available: http://www.jstor.org/stable/4533952

18. Harrington FH. Aggressive howling in wolves. Anim Behav. Academic Press; 1987;35: 7–12. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80204-X

19. Schassburger RM. Vocal communication in the timber wolf, Canis lupus, Linnaeus: structure, motivation, and ontogeny; with 6 tables. Parey Scientific Publ.; 1993.

20. Tooze ZJ, Harrington FH, Fentress JC. Individually distinct vocalizations in timber wolves, Canis lupus. Anim Behav. 1990;40: 723–730. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80701-8

21. Palacios V, Font E, Márquez R. Iberian wolf howls: acoustic structure, individual variation, and a comparison with north american populations. J Mammal. 2007;88: 606–613. doi: 10.1644/06-MAMM-A-151R1.1

22. Root-Gutteridge H, Bencsik M, Chebli M, Gentle LK, Terrell-Nield C, Bourit A, et al. Improving individual identification in captive Eastern grey wolves (Canis lupus lycaon) using the time course of howl amplitudes. Bioacoustics-the Int J Anim Sound Its Rec. 2014;23: 39–53. doi: 10.1080/09524622.2013.817318

23. Root-Gutteridge H, Bencsik M, Chebli M, Gentle LK, Terrell-Nield C, Bourit A, et al. Identifying individual wild Eastern grey wolves (Canis lupus lycaon) using fundamental frequency and amplitude of howls. Bioacoustics Int J Anim Sound Its Rec. 2014;23: 55–66. doi: 10.1080/09524622.2013.817317

24. Watson SK, Townsend SW, Range F. Wolf howls encode both sender- and context-speci fi c information. Anim Behav. Elsevier Ltd; 2018;145: 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.09.005

25. Zaccaroni M, Passilongo D, Buccianti A, Dessi-Fulgheri F, Facchini C, Gazzola A, et al. Group specific vocal signature in free-ranging wolf packs. Ethol Ecol Evol. Taylor & Francis; 2012;24: 322–331.

26. Hennelly LH, Habib B, Root-Gutteridge H, Palacios V, Passilongo D. Howl variation across Himalayan, North African, Indian, and Holarctic wolf clades: tracing divergence in the world ‘ s oldest wolf lineages using acoustics. Curr Zool. 2017; 1–8. doi: 10.1093/cz/zox001 29491993

27. Coscia EM, Phillips DP, Fentress JC. Spectral analysis of neonatal wolf canis lupus vocalizations. Bioacoustics. 1991;3: 275–293. doi: 10.1080/09524622.1991.9753190

28. McCarley H. Vocalizations of Red Wolves (Canis rufus). J Mammal. Oxford University Press; 1978;59: 27–35. doi: 10.1644/859.1.Key

29. Cohen JA, Fox MW. Vocalizations in wild canids and possible effects of domestication. Behav Processes. 1976;1: 77–92. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(76)90008-5 24923546

30. Faragó T, Townsend S, Range F. The Information Content of Wolf (and Dog) Social Communication. In: Witzany G, editor. Biocommunication of Animals. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2014. pp. 41–62. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-7414-8_4

31. Mech DL. The wolf: the ecology and behavior of endangered species. New York: University of Minnesota Press; 1981.

32. Mech DL, Boitani L. Wolves: behavior, ecology, and conservation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2010.

33. Feddersen-Petersen DU. Vocalization of European wolves (Canis lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canis lupus f. fam.). Arch für Tierzucht. 2000;43: 387–397. doi: 10.5194/aab-43-387-2000

34. Mech DL, Boitani L. Wolves: behavior, ecology and conservation. University of Chicago Press, Chicago; 2003.

35. Joslin P. Summer activities of two timber wolf (Canis lupus) packs in Algonquin Park. University of Toronto. 1966.

36. Fentress JC. Observations on the Behavioral Development of a Hand-Reared Male Timber Wolf. 1967;351: 339–351.

37. Nikolʹskij AA, Frommolʹt K-C. Zvukovaja aktivnostʹ volka: Lautaktivität des Wolfes. Izdatelʹstvo Moskovskogo universiteta; 1989.

38. Mech LD. wolves of Isle Royale. 1966;

39. Passilongo D, Marchetto M, Apollonio M. Singing in a wolf chorus: Structure and complexity of a multicomponent acoustic behaviour. Hystrix, Ital J Mammal Online. 2017;28: 180–185. doi: 10.4404/hystrix-28.2–12019

40. Habib B. Ecology of Indian wolf [canis lupus pallipes sykes. 1831), and modeling its potential habitat in the great Indian bustard sanctuary, Maharashtra, India. Aligarh Muslim University, Aligarh (India). 2007.

41. Singh M, Kumara HN. Distribution, status and conservation of Indian gray wolf (Canis lupus pallipes) in Karnataka, India. J Zool. 2006;270: 164–169. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00103.x

42. Jethva BD, Jhala Y V. Foraging ecology, economics and conservation of Indian wolves in the Bhal region of Gujarat, Western India. Biol Conserv. 2004;116: 351–357. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3207(03)00218-0

43. Habib B, Kumar S. Den shifting by wolves in semi-wild landscapes in the Deccan Plateau, Maharashtra, India. J Zool. 2007;272: 259–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00265.x

44. Kumar S, Rahmani AR. Predation by wolves (Canis lupus pallipes) on blackbuck (Antilope cervicapra) in the Great Indian Bustard Sanctuary, Nannaj, Maharashtra, India. Int J Ecol Environ Sci. 2008;34: 99–112.

45. Rodgers WA, Panwar SH. Biogeographical classification of India. New For Dehra Dun, India. 1988;

46. Reddy CS, Jha CS, Diwakar PG, Dadhwal VK. Nationwide classification of forest types of India using remote sensing and GIS. Environ Monit Assess. Springer International Publishing; 2015;187: 777. doi: 10.1007/s10661-015-4990-8 26615560

47. Bioacoustics Research Program. Raven Pro: interactive sound analysis software [Internet]. The Cornell Lab of Ornithology. Ithaca, NY: The Cornell Lab of Ornithology.; 2014. Available: http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven

48. Rader CM. Discrete Fourier transforms when the number of data samples is prime. Proc IEEE. 1968;56: 1107–1108. doi: 10.1109/PROC.1968.6477

49. Rohatgi A. WebPlotDigitizer. Austin, Texas, USA; 2017.

50. Smith LI. A tutorial on Principal Components Analysis Introduction. Statistics (Ber). 2002;51: 52. doi: 10.1080/03610928808829796

51. Kaiser HF. Computer Program for Varimax Rotation in Factor Analysis. Educ Psychol Meas. 1959;19: 413–420. doi: 10.1177/001316445901900314

52. Maechler M, Rousseeuw P, Struyf A, Hubert M, Hornik K. cluster: Cluster Analysis Basics and Extensions. 2019.

53. Kaufman L, Rousseeuw PJ. Agglomerative Nesting (Program AGNES). Finding Groups in Data. Wiley; 2009. pp. 199–252.

54. Rousseeuw PJ. Silhouettes: A graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. J Comput Appl Math. 1987;20: 53–65. doi: 10.1016/0377-0427(87)90125-7

55. Galili T. dendextend: an R package for visualizing, adjusting, and comparing trees of hierarchical clustering. Bioinformatics. 2015; doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btv428 26209431

56. Kaiser HF. Coefficient Alpha for a Principal Component and the Kaiser-Guttman Rule. Psychol Rep. SAGE PublicationsSage CA: Los Angeles, CA; 1991;68: 855–858. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1991.68.3.855

57. Crisler L. Arctic Wild. London: Secker & Warburg; 1959.

58. Weir JN. The contexts and sound of the squeaking vocalization of wolves (Canis lupus) [Internet]. Memorial University of Newfoundland. 1999. Available: https://research.library.mun.ca/9649/

59. Goldman JA, Phillips DP, Fentress JC. An acoustic basis for maternal (Canis lupus)? recognition in timber wolves. 1995;97: 1970–1973. doi: 10.1121/1.412070 7699177

60. Buxton R, Lendrum P, Crooks KR, Wittemyer G. Pairing camera traps and acoustic recorders to monitor the ecological impact of human disturbance. Glob Ecol Conserv. Elsevier; 2018; e00493.

61. Stevenson BC, Borchers DL, Altwegg R, Swift RJ, Gillespie DM, Measey GJ. A general framework for animal density estimation from acoustic detections across a fixed microphone array. Methods Ecol Evol. Wiley Online Library; 2015;6: 38–48.

62. Fuller TK, Sampson BA. Evaluation of a simulated howling survey for wolves. J Wildl Manage. JSTOR; 1988; 60–63.

63. Papin M, Aznar M, Germain E, Guérold F, Pichenot J. Using acoustic indices to estimate wolf pack size. Ecol Indic. Elsevier; 2019;103: 202–211. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolind.2019.03.010

64. Palacios V, López-bao JV, Llaneza L, Fernández C. Decoding Group Vocalizations: The Acoustic Energy Distribution of Chorus Howls Is Useful to Determine Wolf Reproduction. 2016; 1–12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153858 27144887

65. Passilongo D, Mattioli L, Bassi E, Szabó L, Apollonio M. Visualizing sound: counting wolves by using a spectral view of the chorus howling. Front Zool. Frontiers in Zoology; 2015;12: 12–22. doi: 10.1186/s12983-015-0101-5

66. Harrington FH, Mech DL. Wolf Vocalization. Wolf and Man. Elsevier; 1978. pp. 109–132. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-319250-9.50014–1

67. Macdonald D., & Sillero-Zubiri C. The Biology and Conservation of Wild Canids [Internet]. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198515562.001.0001

68. Manser MB, Jansen DA, Graw B, Hollén LI, Bousquet CAH, Furrer RD, et al. Vocal complexity in meerkats and other mongoose species. Advances in the Study of Behavior. Elsevier; 2014. pp. 281–310.

69. Holekamp KE, Boydston EE, Szykman M, Graham I, Nutt KJ, Birch S, et al. Vocal recognition in the spotted hyaena and its possible implications regarding the evolution of intelligence. Anim Behav. Elsevier; 1999;58: 383–395.

70. Pollard KA, Blumstein DT. Evolving communicative complexity: insights from rodents and beyond. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. The Royal Society; 2012;367: 1869–1878.


Článok vyšiel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 10
Najčítanejšie tento týždeň
Najčítanejšie v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvýšte si kvalifikáciu online z pohodlia domova

Aktuální možnosti diagnostiky a léčby litiáz
nový kurz
Autori: MUDr. Tomáš Ürge, PhD.

Všetky kurzy
Prihlásenie
Zabudnuté heslo

Zadajte e-mailovú adresu, s ktorou ste vytvárali účet. Budú Vám na ňu zasielané informácie k nastaveniu nového hesla.

Prihlásenie

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte sa

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#